Bold font designates new information since
last revision.
Baker, T. J., & Bichsel, J. (2006).
Personality predictors of intelligence: Differences between young
and cognitively healthy older adults. Personality and
Individual Differences, 41(5), 861-871. (click to
view)
Abstract. Previous investigations of
personality–intelligence relationships have sampled mainly
young adults. The present study compared young and older groups in
identifying personality predictors of cognitive abilities. A sample
of 381 adults was administered the Woodcock–Johnson III Tests
of Cognitive Abilities and the Big Five Inventory- 44. Participants
were separated into three groups: young adults (aged 19–60),
older adults that were cognitively comparable to the young, and
cognitively superior older adults. Results indicated that Openness
and Extraversion predicted cognitive abilities in the young and
cognitively comparable old, but the speci.c abilities predicted
were di.erent for the two groups. In the cognitively superior older
group, Agreeableness was a negative predictor of Gc
(b
= .28), and Conscientiousness and Openness were
predictors of short- term memory and visual and auditory
processing.
Benson, N. (2008).
Cattell-Horn-Carroll cognitive abilities and reading
achievement. Journal of Psychoeducational
Assessment, 26(1), 27-4.(click
to view)
Abstract: Structural equation modeling
procedures are applied to the standardization sample of the
Woodcock–Johnson III to simultaneously estimate the effects
of a psychometric general factor (g), specific cognitive abilities,
and reading skills on reading achievement. The results of this
study indicate that g has a strong direct relationship with basic
reading skills until about sixth grade. Also, g is found to have a
strong indirect effect on reading fluency and comprehension across
grade levels. Basic reading skills has a strong direct effect on
reading fluency across grade levels. The effect of cognitive
processing speed (Gs) on reading fluency increase with age. Reading
fluency initially has a strong direct effect on reading
comprehension, but this effect is reduced with age. Conversely, the
direct effect of crystallized intelligence or knowledge (Gc) on
reading comprehension increase with age.
Beron, K. J., & .Farkas, G. (2004). Oral
Language and Reading Success: A Structural Equation Modeling
Approach.Structural Equation Modeling, 11( 1),
110-132. (click to
view)
.
Abstract: Oral language skills and habits may serve as
important resources for success or failure in school-
related tasks such as learning
to read. This article tests this hypothesis utilizing a unique data
set, the original Woodcock- Johnson psycho- Educational
Battery- Revised norming sample. This article assesses the
importance of oral language by focusing on auditory processing, a
variable strongly affected by the oral language of the family and
peer group within which the youth is raised. It estimates a
structural equation model in which this variable, along with other
measures of basic cognitive skills, serve as mediators between race
and mother's schooling background and basic and advanced reading
skill. The model fits very well, and the youth's basic skill at
auditory processing is both a major determinant of basic reading
success, and by far the most important of the mediating variables.
In particular, for children ages 5 to 10, this measure accounts for
much of the race effect, and for more than one half of the mother's
education effect on reading. Research on the determinants of social
inequality should pay greater attention to the central importance
of family and peer group oral language in determining cognitive
performance outcomes, particularly for elementary school aged
children
Buckhalt, J., McGhee, R., & Ehrler, D.
(2001). An investigation of Gf-Gc theory in the older adult
population: Joint factor analysis of the Woodcock- Johnson-Revised
and the Detroit Test of Learning Aptitude-Adult.
Psychological Reports, 88, 1161-1170.
Eastwood, A. E. (2002). Memory or attention?
Understanding working memory in children. Dissertation
Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences & Engineering,
62((12-B)), 5986.
Abstract: This dissertation explores the construct of
working memory (WM) in children, defined as the ability to
concurrently remember and process information over brief periods of
time. The research presented here had several goals with respect to
children's working memory: (1) to develop tests of working memory
that have adequate psychometric properties; (2) to ascertain
whether working memory is distinct from short term memory; and (3)
to investigate the relative contributions of processing speed (PS),
controlled attention (CA), and short- term memory (STM) in
accounting for individual differences in working memory capacity.
To address these questions, tests thought to measure WM, STM, CA,
and PS were administered to 119 normally functioning children
between the ages of nine and thirteen. Two working memory tasks
were modeled after the work of Daneman and Carpenter (1980), Engle,
Carullo and Collins (1991), and Salthouse, Mitchell, Skovronek and
Babcock (1989), that involved concurrent storage and
semantic/computational processing of orally presented
sentences/arithmetic calculations. The new WM measures were shown
to have adequate internal consistency but inadequate test-retest
reliability. CA was operationalized using the Stroop Colour and
Word Test, the Trail- making Test, and commission errors on the
Continuous Performance Test. STM was measured using the California
Verbal Learning Test and the Semantic Categorization subtest from
the Swanson Cognitive Processing Test. PS was assessed using the
Visual Matching subtest from the Woodcock-Johnson tests of
Cognitive ability, and the Symbol Search subtest from the Wechsler
Intelligence Scale for Children, Third Edition. Structural equation
modeling techniques were used to investigate the relations between
working memory and other cognitive abilities. The results indicated
that WM is distinct from, though strongly correlated with, STM.
Path models indicated that this correlation is largely a function
of individual differences in controlled attention, which accounts
for about half of the variance in the latent WM factor. Tests of PS
and CA were found to best fit a one- factor solution. Because PS
and CA were very highly correlated (.96, and therefore,
indistinguishable) in the present sample, it was not possible to
test predictions about how they would interact with each other in
the prediction of WM. The implications of this result with respect
to understanding individual differences in WM capacity are
discussed. Overall, the results of the present study are consistent
with Engle, Tuholski, Laughlin and Conway's (1999) model in
suggesting that CA is a significant predictor of WM capacity.
Indeed, when one accounts for CA, STM appears to add little to the
prediction of WM capacity.
Evans, J. J., Floyd, R. G., McGrew, K. S.,
&Leforgee, M. H. (2002). The relations between measures of
Cattell- Horn- Carroll (CHC) cognitive abilities and reading
achievement during childhood and adolescence. School
Psychology Review, 31(2), 246- 262. (click to
view)
Abstract: This study examined the relations between
the Cattell-Horn- Carroll (CHC) theory of Cognitive abilities and
reading achievement during childhood and adolescence. In a large,
nationally representative sample including students 6 to 19 years
of age, operational measures of CHC cognitive abilities obtained
from the Woodcock- Johnson III (WJ III, Woodcock, McGrew,
&Mather, 2001) were found to be signficantly related to the
components of reading achievement. Multiple repssion analyses were
used to regress several WJ III cognitive clusters onto the WJ III
Basic Reading Skills and Reading Comprehension clusters for 14 age
groups. Comprehension- Knowledge (Gc) demonstrated moderate to
strong relations with the components of reading achievement across
childhood and adolescence, and Short-term Memory (Gsm) demonstrated
moderate relations throughout this period. Auditory Process-ing
(Gu), Long-term Retrieval (Glr), and Processing Speed (Gs)
demonstrated moderate relations with the components of reading
achievement during the elementary school years. More specialized
cognitive clusters (viz., PhonemicAwareness and Working Memory)
demonstrated moderate to strong relations. In contrast, Fluid
Reasoning (Gf) and Visual-Spatial Thinking (Gv) demonstrated no
consistent pattern of significant relations across childhood and
adolescence. The results offer external validity evidence for the
WJ III cognitive clusters and provide valuable insights into the
specific cognitive abilities that are important for understanding
the development of reading skills during childhood and
adolescence.
Floyd, R.
G. , Evans, J. J., & McGrew, K. S. (2003). Relations between
measures of Cattell- Horn- Carroll (CHC) cognitive abilities and
mathematics achievement across the school-age years.
Psychology in the Schools, 40(2), 155-
171.(click to
view)
Abstract: Cognitive clusters from the Woodcock-Johnson III
(WJ III) Tests of Cognitive Abilities that measure select Cattell-
Horn-Carroll broad and narrow cognitive abilities were shown
to be significantly related to mathematics achievement in a large,
nationally representative sample of children and adolescents.
Multiple regression analyses were used to predict performance on
the Math Calculation Skills and Math Reasoning clusters from the WJ
III Tests of Achievement for 14 age groups ranging in age from 6 to
19 years. Comprehension- Knowledge (Gc) demonstrated moderate
relations with Math Calculation Skills after the early school-age
years and moderate to strong relations with Math Reasoning. Fluid
Reasoning (Gf ), Short-term Memory (Gsm), and Working Memory
generally demonstrated moderate relations with the mathematics
clusters. Processing Speed (Gs) demonstrated moderate relations
with Math Reasoning during the elementary school years and moderate
to strong relations with Math Calculation Skills. During the
earliest ages of the analysis, Long-term Retrieval (Glr)
demonstrated moderate relations with the mathematics clusters, and
Auditory Processing (Ga) demonstrated moderate relations with Math
Calculation Skills. Visual-Spatial Thinking (Gv) generally
demonstrated nonsignificant relations with the mathematics
clusters.
Floyd,
R., Keith, T., Taub, G. McGrew, K. (2007).
Cattell–Horn–Carroll Cognitive Abilities and Their
Effects on Reading Decoding Skills: g Has Indirect Effects, More
Specific Abilities Have Direct Effects. School
Psychology Quarterly, 22(2), 200-
223.(click to
view)
Abstract: This study employed
structural equation modeling to examine the effects of
Cattell–Horn–Carroll (CHC) abilities on reading
decoding skills using five age- differentiated subsamples from the
standardization sample of the Woodcock–Johnson III (Woodcock,
McGrew, & Mather, 2001). Using the Spearman Model including
only g, strong direct effects of g on reading decoding skills were
demonstrated at all ages. Using the Two-Stratum Model including g
and broad abilities, direct effects of the broad abilities
Long-Term Storage and Retrieval, Processing Speed, Crystallized
Intelligence, Short- Term Memory, and Auditory Processing on
reading decoding skills were demonstrated at select ages. Using the
Three-Stratum Model including g, broad abilities, and narrow
abilities, direct effects of the broad ability Processing Speed and
the narrow abilities Associative Memory, Listening Ability, General
Information, Memory Span, and Phonetic Coding were demonstrated at
select ages. Across both the Two- Stratum Model and the
Three-Stratum Model at all ages, g had very large but indirect
effects. The findings suggest that school psychologists should
interpret measures of some specific cognitive abilities when
conducting psychoeducational assessments designed to explain
reading decoding skills.
Floyd, R., McGrew, K. & Evans, J.
(2008). The relative contributions of the Cattell-Horn-
Carroll cognitive abilities in explaining writing achievement
during childhood and adolescence. Psychology in the Schools,
45(2), 132-144. (click to
view)
Abstract: This study examined the
relative contributions of measures of Cattell- Horn- Carroll (CHC)
cognitive abilities in explaining writing achievement. Drawing from
samples that covered the age range of 7 to 18 years, simultaneous
multiple regression was used to regress scores from the Woodcock-
Johnson III (WJ III; Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001) that
represent CHC broad and narrow abilities onto the WJ III Basic
Writing Skills and Written Expression cluster scores. At most age
levels, Comprehension- Knowledge demonstrated moderate to
strong effects on both writing clusters, Processing Speed
demonstrated moderate effects on Basic Writing Skills and moderate
to strong effects on Written Expression, and Short- Term Memory
demonstrated moderate effects. At the youngest age levels, Long-
Term Retrieval demonstrated moderate to strong effects on Basic
Writing Skills and moderate effects on Written Expression. Auditory
Processing, and Phonemic Awareness demonstrated moderate effects on
only Written Expression at the youngest age levels and at some of
the oldest age levels. Fluid Reasoning demonstrated moderate
effects on both writing clusters only during some of the oldest age
levels. Visual- Spatial Thinking primarily demonstrated negligible
effects. The results provide insights into the cognitive abilities
most important for understanding the writing skills of children
during the school-age years.
Luo, D., Thompson, L. A., &
Detterman, D. K. (2006). The criterion validity of tasks of basic
cognitive processes.Intelligence, 34(1),
79-120.(click to
view)
Abstract. The present study evaluated the
criterion validity of the aggregated tasks of basic cognitive
processes (TBCP). In age groups from 6 to 19 of the
Woodcock-Johnson III Cognitive Abilities and Achievement Tests
normative sample, the aggregated TBCP, i.e., the processing speed
and working memory clusters, correlate with measures of scholastic
achievement as strongly as the conventional indexes of crystallized
intelligence and fluid intelligence. These basic processing
aggregates also mediate almost exhaustively the correlations
between measures of fluid intelligence and achievement, and appear
to explain substantially more of the achievement measures than the
fluid ability index. The results from the Western Reserve Twin
Project sample using TBCP with more rigorous experimental paradigms
were similar, suggesting that it may be practically feasible to
adopt TBCP with experimental paradigms into the psychometric
testing tradition. Results based on the latent factors in
structural equation models largely confirmed the findings based on
the observed aggregates and composites.
McGrew,
K., Keith, T., Flanagan, D. & Vanderwood, M.
(1997). Beyond g: The impact of Gf- Gc specific
cognitive abilities research on the future use and interpretation
of intelligence tests in the schools. School
Psychology Review, 26(2), 189-
210. (click to
view).
McGrew, K. & Hessler, G. (1995). The
relationship between the WJ-R Gf-Gc cognitive clusters and
mathematics achievement across the life-span.
Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment,
13, 21- 38.(click to
view)
Miller, B. D. (2001). Utilizing
Cattell-Horn-Carroll Cross-Battery assessment to predict reading
achievement in learning disabled middle school students.
Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: Humanities
& Social Sciences, 61((12- A)), 4673.
Abstract: Recent research has pointed out the
theoretical basis and predictive utility of Cattell- Horn-
Carroll (CHC) theory in cognitive assessment. Guidelines have
also been presented for conducting cross-battery assessments using
CHC theory as a basis. The general purpose of the present study was
to utilize CHC- based Cross-Battery Assessment to predict reading
achievement as measured by traditional or alternative forms of
assessment, such as Curriculum- Based Measurement (CBM). Hypotheses
formulated for the present study included auditory processing,
crystallized intelligence, short term memory, long term retrieval,
processing speed, and fluid intelligence being important predictors
of basic reading skills and reading comprehension in learning
disabled middle school students. The present study was conducted
through a records review. All students received psychological
evaluations and were assessed through CHC Cross- Battery
Assessment. In addition, educational evaluations consisting of the
Woodcock- Johnson Tests of Achievement-Revised and three CBM oral
reading fluency probes were also administered to the students.
Stepwise multiple regressions were performed for each of three
dependent variables (word identification, passage comprehension,
and oral reading fluency) using six independent variables (fluid
intelligence, crystallized intelligence, short term memory,
auditory processing, long term retrieval, and processing speed).
Results at least partially substantiated the hypotheses that
crystallized intelligence, processing speed, auditory processing,
and short term memory would be significant predictors of basic
reading skills and reading comprehension in learning disabled
middle school students. However, hypotheses that long term
retrieval and fluid intelligence would be significant predictors of
basic reading skills and reading comprehension in learning disabled
middle school students were not substantiated. Implications for
future research and school psychology practice were also
discussed.
Serpell,
R. , Sonnenschein, S., Baker, L., &Ganapathy, H. (2002).
Intimate culture of families in the early socialization of
literacy. Journal of Family Psychology, 16(4),
391-405. (click to
view)
Abstract: The intimate family culture for early
literacy socialization was documented for a socioculturally
heterogeneous sample of 66 children enrolled in pre-kindergarten
through third grade at public elementary schools in a large U.S.
city. Parents were interviewed about 3 types of indexes of their
family's intimate culture: the child's engagement in various
literacy-related activities at home, the parents' orientation
towards the significance of literacy for early child development,
and the family's routines of dinnertime, reading aloud, and doing
homework for school. Basic reading competencies were assessed with
the Woodcock- Johnson Psychoeducational Battery-- Tests of
Achievement, Revised. Multiple regression analysis found that a
significant proportion of variance in the children's literacy
development was predicted by each of the quantitative indexes of
intimate family culture, leaving little or no additional variance
that was due to family income or ethnicity.
Taub, G., Floyd, R., Keith, T. &
McGrew, K. (2008). Effects of General and Broad Cognitive
Abilities on Mathematics Achievement. School Psychology
Quarterly, 23 (2), 187–198. (click to
view)
Abstract: This study investigated the direct
and indirect effects of general intelligence and 7 broad cognitive
abilities on mathematics achievement. Structural equation modeling
was used to investigate the simultaneous effects of both general
and broad cognitive abilities on students’ mathematics
achievement. A hierarchical model of intelligence derived from the
Cattell–Horn–Carroll (CHC) taxonomy of intelligence was
used for all analyses. The participants consisted of 4
age-differentiated subsamples (ranging from ages 5 to 19) from the
standardization sample of the Woodcock–Johnson III (WJ III;
Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001). Data from each of the 4 age-
differentiated subsamples were divided into 2 data sets. At each
age level, one data set was used for model testing and
modification, and a second data set was used for model validation.
The following CHC broad cognitive ability factors demonstrated
statistically significant direct effects on the mathematics
achievement variables: Fluid Reasoning, Crystallized Intelligence,
and Processing Speed. In contrast, across all age levels, the
general intelligence factor demonstrated indirect effects on the
mathematics achievement variable.
Vanderwood, M. L., McGrew, K. S., Flanagan, D. P., &Keith,
T. Z. (2002). The contribution of general and specific cognitive
abilities to reading achievement. Learning and Individual
Differences, 13, 159- 188. (click to
view)
Abstract. Since the development of the Weschler
scales, significant advances have been made in intelligence theory
and testing technology that have the potential to provide a more
comprehensive understanding of cognitive abilities than currently
exists. For this study, the standardization sample of the
Woodcock–Johnson Psychoeducational Battery- Revised
(WJ- R)—an empirically supported measure of several
constructs within the Cattell–Horn–Carroll (CHC) theory
of cognitive abilities—was used to analyze the contribution
of specific cognitive abilities to reading achievement at five
developmental levels. Structural equation modeling (SEM), with
calibration and cross- validation samples, of four different models
of the hypothesized relations among the variables was conducted to
determine if specific abilities can provide relevant information
regarding the development of reading skills. The results of this
study clearly indicate that Gc (comprehension knowledge or
crystallized intelligence) and Ga (auditory processing) play an
important role in the development of reading skills.
Vellutino, F., Tunmer, W., Jaccard, J.
& Chen, R. (2007). Components of Reading Ability:
Multivariate Evidence for a Convergent Skills Model of Reading
Development. Scientific Studies of Reading,
11(1), 3-32. (click to
view).
Elementary and middle school children were given a large
battery of tests evaluating reading subskills and reading- related
cognitive abilities. These measures were used to define latent
constructs representing skills and abilities believed to be
important components of reading comprehension. Hypothesized
relationships among these constructs were specified within the
context of a structural model we call the “Convergent Skills
Model of Reading Development,” and developmental differences
in the relative contribution made by each construct to reading
comprehension performance were assessed through confirmatory factor
analysis using the LISREL and AMOS programs. Results provide
qualified support for the model and were interpreted as consistent
with the major premises of both Gough and Tunmer’s (1986)
“Simple View” and Sticht’s (1979)
“Audread” models of reading.
Yeung, W.,
Linver, M. R., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2002). How money matters for
young children's development: Parental investment and family
processes. Child Development, 73(6 ),
1861-1879. (click to
view)
Abstract: This study used data from the Panel Study of
Income Dynamics and its 1997 Child Development Supplement to
examine how family income matters for young children's development.
The sample included 753 children (aged 3-5 yrs) in 1997. Two sets
of mediating factors were examined that reflect two dominating
views in the literature: (1) the investment perspective, and (2)
the family process perspective. The study examined how two measures
of income (stability and level) were associated with preschool
children's developmental outcomes (Woodcock-Johnson [W- J]
Achievement Test scores and the Behavior Problem Index [BPI])
through investment and family process pathways. Results supported
the hypothesis that distinct mediating mechanisms operate on the
association between income and different child outcomes. Much of
the association between income and children's W-
J scores was mediated by the family's ability to
invest in providing a stimulating learning environment. In
contrast, family income was associated with children's BPI scores
primarily through maternal emotional distress and parenting
practices. Level of income was associated with W-J letter-word
scores and income stability was associated with W-J applied problem
scores and BPI, even after all controls were included in the
models.
|
|