Bold font designates new information not in
prior version.
Beaujean, A., Knoop, A. & Holliday, G.
(2006). Does chronometry have a place in assessing math
disorders? Learning Disability Quarterly, 29, 32-38. (click
to view).
Birch, K. G. (2004). Phonological processing,
automaticity, auditory processing, and memory in slow learners and
children with reading disabilities. Dissertation
Abstracts International Section A: Humanities & Social
Sciences, 64(12- A), 4351. (click to
view)
Abstract: The definition and classification of
learning disabilities has been an ongoing debate, largely related
to the use of a discrepancy between intelligence and achievement as
the determining factor. Children who have a discrepancy are
typically classified as learning disabled and qualify for services
through the schools, while those who have difficulty reading but do
not have a discrepancy due to low or below average intelligence
levels ("slow learners") are frequently denied services. Many
studies have revealed more similarities than differences between
slow learners and children with learning disabilities, yet
educational policy has not reflected these findings. In order to
further the understanding of differences between the two groups and
to provide additional information about the reading process, the
current study examined the relationship between phonological
processing, automaticity, auditory processing, and memory in slow
learners and children with reading disabilities. Participants were
selected from a sample of 2,361 students in the first through fifth
grades who were tested as a part of the standardization for the
Woodcock- Johnson III. Three groups were formed: Control (n = 75),
Slow Learner (n = 79), and Learning Disabled (n = 32), resulting in
a total sample size of 186 participants. MANOVA results revealed
overall differences between the groups. Follow-up comparisons found
that the Slow Learner group had significantly poorer performance as
compared to the Control group on all measures; the Learning
Disabled group was significantly worse than the Control group on
Phonological Processing but not on any other measure; and the Slow
Learner Group performed significantly worse than the Learning
Disabled group on all measures. Correlational analyses revealed a
series of significant correlations from small to large. Results
from a multiple regression revealed that from the four factors that
were investigated, Phonological Processing was the only
statistically significant contributor to the variance of Basic
Reading. Results appear to support the "phonological-core-
variable- difference" hypothesis from the literature (Stanovich,
1988), as well as current proposals for changes to educational
policy. Future research should be directed towards determining the
capacity of slow learners and children with learning disabilities
to respond to intervention.
Butterbaugh, G., Olejniczak, P., Roques B.,
Costa R. , Rose, M., Fisch, B., Carey, M., Thomson, J., &
Skinner, J. (2004). Lateralization of temporal lobe epilepsy and
learning disabilities, as defined by disability- related civil
rights law.Epilepsia, 45(8 ), 963-
970.
Abstract: Epilepsy research has identified higher
rates of learning disorders in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy
(TLE). However, most studies have not adequately assessed complex
functional adult learning skills, such as reading comprehension and
written language. We designed this study to evaluate our
predictions that higher rates of reading comprehension, written
language, and calculation disabilities would be associated with
left TLE versus right TLE. METHODS: Reading comprehension, written
language, and calculation skills were assessed by using selected
subtests from the Woodcock- Johnson Psycho- Educational Tests of
Achievement- Revised in a consecutive series of 31 presurgical
patients with TLE. Learning disabilities were defined by one
essential criterion consistent with the Americans with Disabilities
Act of 1990. Patients had left hemisphere language dominance based
on Wada results, left or right TLE based on inpatient EEG
monitoring, and negative magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), other
than MRI correlates of mesial temporal sclerosis. RESULTS: Higher
rates of reading comprehension, written language, and calculation
disabilities were associated with left TLE, as compared with right
TLE. Nearly 75% of patients with left TLE, whereas fewer than 10%
of those with right TLE, had at least one learning disability.
CONCLUSIONS: Seizure onset in the language- dominant hemisphere, as
compared with the nondominant hemisphere, was associated with
higher rates of specific learning disabilities and a history of
poor literacy or career development or both. These results support
the potential clinical benefits of using lateralization of seizure
onset as a predictor of the risk of learning disabilities that,
once evaluated, could be accommodated to increase the participation
of patients with epilepsy in work and educational
settings.(click to
view)
Camarata,
S. & Woodcock, R. (2006; in press). Sex differences in
processing speed: Developmental effects in males and
females. Intelligence. (click to view
uncorrected page proofs)
Cirino, P., Morris, K. & Morris, R. Semantic,
Executive, and Visuospatial Abilities in Mathematical Reasoning of
Referred College Students.Assessment, 14
(1), 94-104. (click to
view).
Semantic retrieval (SR) and executive-procedural (EP), but
not visuospatial (VS) skills, have been found to be uniquely
predictive of mathematical calculation skills in a sample of
clinically referred college students. This study set out to cross-
validate these results in an independent sample of clinically
referred college students (N = 337) as well as extend them by
examination of the contributions of these cognitive domains to math
reasoning skills. Results indicate that these cognitive domains
were able to predict 30% of the variance in calculation skills and
50% of the variance in math reasoning; however, in both cases, only
the domains of semantic retrieval and visuospatial skill
contributed uniquely. Differences between studies, and the lack of
unique contribution of the EP domain to either type of math skill,
may be due to measurement and sampling differences, the degree of
shared relations among domains, and the choice of measures that
represent the EP domain. Implications and future directions are
explored.
Edwards. O. & Oakland, T. (2006). Factorial Invariance of
Woodcock-Johnson III Scores for African Americans and
Caucasian. Journal of Psychoeducational
Assessment, 24 (4), 358- 366. (click
to view)
Abstract: Bias in testing has been of
interest to psychologists and other test users since the origin of
testing. New or revised tests often are subject to analyses that
help examine the degree of bias in reference to group membership
based on gender, language use, and race/ethnicity. The pervasive
use of intelligence test data when making critical and, at times,
life-changing decisions warrants the need by test developers and
test users to examine possible test bias on new and recently
revised intelligence tests. This study investigates factorial
invariance and criterion related validity of the Woodcock- Johnson
III for African American and Caucasian American students. Data from
this study suggest that although their mean scores differ,
Woodcock- Johnson III scores have comparable meaning for both
groups.
Floyd, R.,
Bergeron, R. &Alfanso, V (2006). Cattell-Horn-Carroll cognitive
ability profiles of poor reading
comprehenders. Reading and Writing,
19,427-456. (click to
view)
Fuchs,
L., Fuchs, D., Stuebing, K., Fletcher, J., Hamlett, C. &
Lambert, W. (2008). Problem solving and computational skill:
Are they shared or distinct aspects of mathematical
cognition? Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(1), 30-
47. (click to
view)
Abstract: The purpose of this study
was to explore patterns of difficulty in 2 domains of mathematical
cognition: computation and problem solving. Third graders
(n 924; 47.3% male) were representatively
sampled from 89 classrooms; assessed on computation and problem
solving; classified as having difficulty with computation, problem
solving, both domains, or neither domain; and measured on 9
cognitive dimensions. Difficulty occurred across domains with the
same prevalence as difficulty with a single domain; specific
difficulty was distributed similarly across domains. Multivariate
profile analysis on cognitive dimensions and chi-square tests on
demographics showed that specific computational difficulty was
associated with strength in language and weaknesses in attentive
behavior and processing speed; problem- solving difficulty was
associated with deficient language as well as race and poverty.
Implications for understanding mathematics competence and for the
identification and treatment of mathematics difficulties are
discussed.
Hurley, G.
A., & Levinson, E. M. (2002). Profiles of learning disability
subtypes in a retrospective sampling of adolescents' scores on the
Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery- Revised.
Psychological Reports, 90(3), 767- 770.
Kalbfleisch, M. L. (2001). Electroencephalographic (EEG)
differences between boys with average and high- aptitude with
and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) during
task transitions. Dissertation Abstracts International:
Section B: The Sciences & Engineering, 62((1- B)),
96.
Abstract: This study extended previous research
performed by Cox, Kovatchev, Morris, Phillips, Hill, and Merkel
(1998), and had three aims: (1) to investigate the Consistency
Index, a quantified measure of electroencephalography (EEG), as a
psychophysiological tool for differentiating between boys with and
without Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), (2) to
identify diagnostic patterns that contribute to an understanding of
the neurobiological etiology of ADHD, and (3) to determine the
relationship between ADHD, IQ, and transitions between divergent
thinking tasks. The CI, a calculation based on Fast Fourier
Transformation of EEG data, measures the neural efficiency of an
individual's brain when shifting between two tasks. Subjects were
screened using the NIMH DISC-IV Parent interview, the DuPaul ADHD
Rating Scale for Home and School, and subtests of the WISC- III
(Block Design, Vocabulary, and Similarities) and Woodcock Johnson
Tests of Achievement (Letter-Word Identification, Dictation, and
Calculation). Subjects with comorbid central nervous system
dysfunction, substance abuse, psychosis, sleep disorder, or those
taking bronchiodialator medication for allergy or asthma were
excluded from the study. In 17 controls and 17 subjects with ADHD
(ages 8-11), shifts were measured in two places: between silent
reading and Subtest 1 of the Torrance Tests for Creative Thinking -
Figural Forms (TTCT), and between Subtests 1 and 3 of the TTCT-
Figural Forms. Mean CI scores discriminated between the control
group and the ADHD/ADD group (p < .01). Mean CI scores also
discriminated between the control group and a subset of the
experimental group with Inattentive- type ADHD (p < .01). The
shift between Subtests 1 and 3 of the TTCT- Figural Forms
Data did not produce significant differences between the groups.
This sample produced a possible EEG profile of ADHD: a
differentiated percentage of theta during active mental processing
when the mean powers of delta, theta, alpha, and beta were compared
from the data used to calculate the CI. IQ did not correlate with
the CI. The CI differentiated statistically between mean scores of
the control and subject groups. Additional research is needed to
hone the CI as a tool that would be clinically useful for aiding in
the diagnosis of ADHD
Keith, T., Reynolds, M., Patel, P. &
Ridley, K. (in press). Sex differences in latent cognitive
abilities ages 6 to 59: Evidence from the Woodcock–Johnson
III tests of cognitive abilities. Intelligence.
(click
to view)
Abstract: Sex differences in the
latent general and broad cognitive abilities underlying the
Woodcock–Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities were
investigated for children, youth, and adults ages 6 through 59. A
developmental, multiple indicator–multiple cause, structural
equation model was used to investigate sex differences in latent
cognitive abilities as well as developmental changes in these
differences across the 6 to 59 age span. Females showed a
consistent advantage on the latent processing speed (Gs) factor,
and males showed a small, consistent advantage on the latent
comprehension–knowledge (Gc) factor. Males also showed an
advantage on latent quantitative reasoning (RQ) and
visual–spatial ability (Gv) factors at most ages, although
the latter was statistically significant only for adults. No
statistically significant sex differences were shown on latent
auditory processing, short-term memory, long-term retrieval, or
fluid reasoning factors. The higher-order, latent g factor showed
inconsistent differences for children, small, nonsignificant
differences favoring females for adolescents, and fairly consistent
statistically significant differences favoring females in
adulthood. Findings are inconsistent with developmental theory that
suggests males should show an advantage on g in adulthood.
Supplemental analyses suggested that methodological choices,
including the use of latent variables versus composites and methods
for dealing with missing data, can affect research
findings.
Kim, S. K., Frisby, C. L., & Davison, M.
L. (2004). Estimating cognitive profiles using profile anlysis via
multidimensional scaling (PAMS). Multivariate Behavioral
Research, 39(4), 595- 624. (click to
view)
Abstract: Two of the most
popular methods of profile analysis, cluster analysis and modal
profile analysis, have limitations. First, neither technique is
adequate when the sample size is large. Second, neither
method will necessarily provide profile information in terms of
both level andpattern. A new method of profile analysis,
called Profile Analysis via Multidimensional Scaling (PAMS;
Davison, 1996), is introduced to meet the challenge. PAMS extends
the use of simple multidimensional scaling methods to identify
latent profiles in a multi-test battery. Application of PAMS
to profile analysis is described. The PAMS model is then used to
identify latent profiles from a subgroup (N = 357) within
the sample of the Woodcock- Johnson Psychoeducational
Battery—Revised(WJ-R; McGrew, Werder, & Woodcock,
1991; Woodcock & Johnson, 1989), followed by a discussion of
procedures for interpreting participants’ observed score
profiles from the latent PAMS profiles. Finally, advantages and
limitations of the PAMS technique are
discussed.
Konold, T., Juel, C.& McKinnon, M.
(1999). Building an integrated model of early reading
acquisition(CIERA Report 1- 1003). Center for the Improvement of
Early Reading Aachievement, University of Michigan.(click to
view)
Murray, C.
(2005, Sept.). The inequality
taboo. Commentary, 13-22. (Note. this
article makes use of the WJ III norm data late in the paper and is
referenced in the footnotes. Click to
view)
Murray,
C. (2007). The magnitude and components of change in the
black–white IQ difference from 1920 to 1991: A birth cohort
analysis of the Woodcock–Johnson standardizations.
Intelligence, 35, 305-318.(click to
view)
Abstract: The black–white
difference in test scores for the three standardizations of the
Woodcock–Johnson battery of cognitive tests is analyzed in
terms of birth cohorts covering the years from 1920 through 1991.
Among persons tested at ages 6–65, a narrowing of the
difference occurred in overall IQ and in the two most highly
g-loaded clusters in the Woodcock–Johnson, Gc and Gf. After
controlling for standardization and interaction effects, the
magnitude of these reductions is on the order of half a standard
deviation from the high point among those born in the 1920s to the
low point among those born in the last half of the 1960s and early
1970s. These reductions do not appear for IQ or Gc if the results
are restricted to persons born from the mid-1940s onward. The
results consistently point to a B–W difference that has
increased slightly on all three measures for persons born after the
1960s. The evidence for a high B–W IQ difference among those
born in the early part of the 20th century and a subsequent
reduction is at odds with other evidence that the B–W IQ
difference has remained unchanged. The end to the narrowing of the
B–W IQ difference for persons born after the 1960s is
consistent with almost all other data that have been analyzed by
birth cohort.
McGee, R., Brodeur, D., Symons, D.,
Andrade, B. &Fahie, C. (2004). Time Perception: Does it
Distinguish ADHD and RD Children in a Clinical Sample?
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32(5),
481–490. (click to
view).
This study used a double-dissociation design to evaluate
whether children with ADHD demonstrated specific deficits relative
to children with Reading Disorders. Recent theory suggests that
ADHD children have deficits in time perception and working memory,
whereas RD children have deficits in phonological decoding. The
performance of 113 clinic- referred children aged 6–11 was
examined using measures of working memory, phonological processing,
and time perception. Respondents completed two time production
tasks in which they were to judge when 30-s had elapsed, and
another in which they were asked to estimate the duration of the
Conners’ CPT (CCPT). Time Perception and phonological
processing variables were submitted to a 2 × 2 ANCOVA (ADHD
vs. RD), covarying for age, SES, IQ, and working memory. Children
with ADHD were more likely to overestimate the time taken for the
CCPT than children without ADHD, but no group differences were
found on the 30-s estimation tasks. Children with RD did not
display deficits in time estimation, but showed deficits in
auditory phonological processing. The lack of interaction effects
supported an “etiological subtype” over the
“phenocopy” model of ADHD and RD. No group differences
were detected using the CCPT. Although our previous studies did not
find an order effect for the Conners’ CPT in a 1-hr battery,
a fatigue effect was evident with a 1.5-hr battery. The
implications for Barkley’s behavioral inhibition theories (R.
Barkley, 1997) are discussed.
Naglieri, J., Rojahn, J. & Matto, H.
(2007). Hispanic and non-Hispanic children's performance on
PASS cognitive processes and achievement.
Intelligence, 35,
568–579. (click
to view).
Abstract: Hispanics have become the
largest minority group in the United States. Hispanic children
typically come from working class homes with parents who have
limited English language skills and educational training. This
presents challenges to psychologists who assess these children
using traditional IQ tests because of the considerable verbal and
academic (e.g., quantitative) content. Some researchers have
suggested that intelligence conceptualized on the basis of
psychological processes may have utility for assessment of children
from culturally and linguistically diverse populations because
verbal and quantitative skills are not included. This study
examined Hispanic children's performance on the Cognitive
Assessment System (CAS; [Naglieri, J.A., and Das, J.P. (1997).
Cognitive Assessment System. Itasca, IL: Riverside.]) which is
based on the Planning, Attention, Simultaneous, and Successive
(PASS) theory of intelligence. The scores of Hispanic (N=244) and
White (N=1956) children on the four PASS processes were obtained
and the respective correlations between PASS and achievement
compared. Three complementary sampling methodologies and data
analysis strategies were chosen to compare the Ethnic groups.
Sample size was maximized using nationally representative groups
and demographic group differences were minimized using smaller
matched samples. Small differences between Hispanic and
non-Hispanic children were found when ability was measured with
tests of basic PASS processes. In addition, the correlation between
the PASS constructs and achievement were substantial for both
Hispanic and non-Hispanic children and were not significantly
different between the groups.
Naglieri,
J. A., & Rojahn, J. (2001). Gender differences in Planning,
Attention, Simultaneous, and Successive (PASS) cognitive processes
and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology,
93(2), 430- 437.(click to
view)
Abstract. Gender differences in ability and
achievement have been studied for some time and have been
conceptualized along verbal, quantitative, and visual-spatial
dimensions. Researchers recently have called for a theory-based
approach to studying these differences. This study examined 1,100
boys and 1,100 girls who matched the U.S. population using the
Planning, Attention, Simultaneous, Successive (PASS) cognitive-
processing theory, built on the neuropsychological work of A. R.
Luria (1973). Girls outperformed boys on the Planning and Attention
scales of the Cognitive Assessment System by about 5 points
(d = .30 and .35, respectively). Gender differences
were also found for a subsample of 1,266 children on the Woodcock-
Johnson Revised Tests of Achievement Proofing
(d = .33), Letter-Word Identification
(d = .22), and Dictation (d = .22). The
results illustrate that the PASS theory offers a useful way to
examine gender differences in cognitive performance
Osmon
D.C., Braun M.M., & Plambeck E.A. (2005). Processing abilities
associated with phonologic and orthographic skills in adult
learning disability. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology, 27(5), 544- 554.(click to
view)
Abstract: This study evaluated college adults (N
= 138) referred for learning problems using a Cattell- Horn-
Carroll based intelligence measure (Woodcock
Johnson- Revised: WJ-R) and spatial and executive function
neuropsychological measures to determine processing abilities
underlying math skills. Auditory and visual perceptual (WJ- R Ga
and Gv), long- and short- memory (WJ-R Glr and Gsm), crystallized
and fluid intellectual (WJ- R Gc and Gf), and spatial and executive
function (Judgment of Line Orientation [JLO] and Category Test)
measures differentiated those with and without math deficits.
Multiple regression revealed selective processing abilities (Gf,
JLO, and Category) predicting about 16% of the variance in math
skills after variance associated with general intelligence (also
about 16%) was removed. Cluster analysis found evidence for a
selective spatial deficit group, a selective executive function
deficit group and a double deficit (spatial and executive function)
group. Results were discussed in relation to a double deficit
hypothesis associated with developmental dyscalculia.
Penny A.
M., Waschbusch D.A, Carrey N, & Drabman, R. S. (2005). Applying
a psychoeducational perspective to ADHD.Journal of
Attention Disorders, 8(4), 208-220.
Abstract: This article examines whether various
cognitive abilities are associated with symptoms of ADHD. Cognitive
ability is conceptualized using Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) theory
as measured using the Woodcock- Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability
(3rd ed.). This article also examines whether test session behavior
mediates the association between cognitive ability and ADHD.
Participants are children ages 6 to 12 with (n= 33) and without (n=
19) ADHD. Results show that inattentive symptoms of ADHD are
significantly related to the CHC ability of processing speed above
and beyond the effect of test session behavior. Symptoms of ADHD
(both inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity) are also
significantly associated with visual spatial processing, but this
is completely mediated by test session behavior. It is concluded
that inattentive symptoms of ADHD are associated with slower
processing speed and that this relationship is not explained by
test session behavior.
Porter, M.
A., & Coltheart, M. (2005). Cognitive heterogeneity in Williams
syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology,
27(2), 275- 306. (click to
view)
Abstract: This study used theWoodcock–Johnson
Tests of Cognitive Ability–Revised to investigate a wide
range of cognitive abilities in people withWilliams syndrome (WS).
It involved a comparatively large sample of 31 people with WS, but
took a case- series approach. The study addressed the widespread
claims of a characteristic “WS cognitive profile” by
looking for heterogeneity rather than homogeneity. People with WS
showed a variety of preserved (significantly above mental age
[MA]), expected (at MA), and significantly impaired (significantly
below MA) levels of functioning. Such results provide clear
evidence for heterogeneity in cognitive functions within WS.We
found the most homogeneity on a test of phonological processing and
a test of phonological short-term memory, with half of the WS
sample performing at MA levels on these tests. Interestingly, noWS
individual showed a weakness on a test of nonverbal reasoning, and
only oneWSindividual showed a weakness on a test of verbal
comprehension. In addition, we found that strengths on
analysis–synthesis and verbal analogies occurred only for WS
individuals with an MA less than 5.5 years (our sample median MA);
people with an MA greater than 5.5 years performed at MA level on
these 2 tests. Results also provided preliminary evidence for
distinct subgroups of WS people based on their cognitive strengths
and weaknesses on a broad range of cognitive functions. On the
basis of the findings, caution should be made in declaring a single
cognitive profile that is characteristic of all individuals with
WS. Just as there is heterogeneity in genetic and physical
anomalies within WS, not all WS individuals share the same
cognitive strengths and weaknesses. Also, not all WS individuals
show the profile of a strength in verbal abilities and a weakness
in spatial functions.
Pratt, J.
J. , Bordieri, J. E., & Taylor, D. W. (2003). The Utility of
the Woodcock-Johnson- Revised for Determining Driver's License
Status among Persons with Cognitive Disabilities.National
Vocational Evaluation & Work Adjustment Journal,
33(1), 9- 17.
Abstract: The purpose of this study was to determine
the utility of the Woodcock- Johnson Test Battery- Revised (WJ-
R) for determining driver's license status among
individuals with cognitive disabilities. A sample group of 147
individuals was chosen from archival file reviews of consumers who
had been administered the WJ-R while receiving driver education
services. Scores obtained on the WJ-R were used to differentiate
driver's license status among these rehabilitation consumers.
Eleven of the 12 WJ- R scores demonstrated significant differences
between licensed and non- licensed consumers.
Proctor, B. E., Floyd, R. G., & Shaver, R.
B. (2005). Cattell-Horn-Carroll broad cognitive ability profiles of
low math achievers. Psychology in the Schools,
42(1), 1-12. (click to
view)
Abstract: This study extends previous
research examining the relations between Cattell- Horn-
Carroll cognitive abilities and math achievement. The cognitive
profiles of children with normative weaknesses in Math Calculation
Skills or Math Reasoning were compared to those of their average-
achieving peers. The cognitive profile of the low Math
Calculation Skills group (n = 68) was similar to that of
their average- achieving peers. The low Math Reasoning group
(n = 52) scored lower than their average- achieving peers on
the cognitive abilities as a set and on Fluid Reasoning and
Comprehension–Knowledge. When individual profiles were
considered, approximately half of the children with normative math
weaknesses demonstrated commensurate weaknesses in one or more
cognitive abilities, which may inform diagnostic models of learning
disabilities.
Proctor, B., & Prevatt, F. (2003).
Agreement Among Four Models Used for Diagnosing Learning
Disabilities.Journal of Learning Disabilities,
36(5), 459- 466. (click to
view)
Abstract: We compared the level of agreement
among four models used to diagnose learning disabilities (LD),
including the simple discrepancy, intraindividual, intellectual
ability–achievement, and underachievement models. The sample
included 170 clinic- referred university students. The simple
discrepancy model diagnosed significantly more students with LD
than the other three models. The highest degree of agreement
occurred between the intraindividual and intellectual
ability–achievement models (70%); the lowest level of
agreement occurred between the simple discrepancy and
underachievement models (48%). Finally, only two of the six
comparisons among the four models demonstrated significant
correlations. We conclude that even when discrepancy models
diagnose similar numbers of students with LD, the
same students are not diagnosed across different
models.
Rahman, J.
R. (2004). Use of the Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive
Ability with gifted children.Dissertation Abstracts
International: Section B: The Sciences & Engineering,
65( 6- B), 3217.
Abstract: Measurement of the abilities of gifted
children is challenging. Identifying patterns of performance for
gifted children has proven elusive. The WJIII COG, a theory- driven
measure, purports to provide information about a child's pattern of
abilities, including areas previously not measured by other
cognitive instruments. Only one study has addressed the
relationship between the WJIII COG and gifted children. (Rizza, et
al., 2001). The purpose of this study was to examine the WJIII
COG's appropriateness for use with gifted children. Interpretive
guidance when using the WJIII COG with gifted children was sought.
The following question was addressed: Do children of high
intellectual ability display distinct patterns of performance on
the WJIII COG Stratum II variables. If so, do these patterns
qualitatively differ from the performance of children of non-high
intellectual ability? The participants for this study were taken
from the standardization sample of WJIII COG participants (N =
3,145). The participants were divided into two groups: high
intellectual ability, Group 1 (N = 389), and "other," (Group 2) (N
= 2756). Group 1 participants had a GIA Standard score of 120 or
above. The participants were further divided into three subgroups,
determined by age: preschool, school-aged, and college- aged.
Approximately half of the sample was male. White participants were
the majority of the sample (77.4- 87.9%). Most participants were
non- Hispanic (91.3-
94.9%). Means, standard
deviations, ranges, minimum values, and maximum values, were
obtained. WJIII COG Stratum II variables were paired and compared.
The comparisons made were ranges and point differences. Cumulative
percentages of point differences were calculated at the 5, 7, 10,
15, 20, 25, 30 point levels. Two primary findings emerged. The
first finding was that Group 1 participants scored higher overall
than Group 2 participants when averages were compared. The second
finding was that no gifted profile emerged for Group 1. Although no
gifted pattern emerged, there was a great deal of variability
within individual student profiles for both Groups. It was
suggested that further research be directed at whether subtypes of
students with high abilities could be identified that would have
clinical implications congruent with multidimensional theories of
giftedness.
Reisetter, T. K. R. (2003). Processing speed
and disabilities in reading. Dissertation Abstracts
International Section A: Humanities & Social Sciences,
64(2-A), 400.
Abstract: This study primarily addressed two
questions. The first question asked if individuals with learning
disabilities in reading differ from individuals with other types of
learning disabilities and from individuals with no clinical
diagnosis on processing speed measured cognitively. The second
question asked if subgroups within the two experimental groups with
learning disabilities could be identified that conform to the
Double Deficit Hypothesis (DDH) (Wolf & Bowers, 2000). Subjects
were extracted from an existing data base that consisted of over
2000 individuals in the Mid- Western United States who had been
referred for neuropsychological assessment, and were chosen for
this study if they had been diagnosed with a learning disability or
had received no clinical diagnosis. A total sample of 307 was
available for the study, including 211 males and 96 females. The
subjects were assigned to one of three groups by the researcher.
One group consisted of individuals diagnosed with learning
disabilities in reading, a second group consisted of individuals
with learning disabilities in areas other than reading, and the
third groups consisted of individuals who had received no clinical
diagnosis. Scores on the Woodcock- Johnson Tests of Cognitive
Ability- Revised, and the Woodcock Johnson Tests of Achievement-
Revised were used for analysis. The investigation into the first
question found that the three groups differed significantly on a
cognitive measure of processing speed. The literature search had
found that an achievement measure of processing speed discriminated
between the three types of individuals. The current findings imply
that processing speed problems may be linked to cognitive abilities
as well as academic abilities. The results for the second question
were not as clear. However, a cluster analysis of both the group
with learning disabilities in reading and the group with learning
disabilities in areas other than reading found processing speed to
be an important factor in describing these individuals'
difficulties. For the first group, Wolf and Bowers (2000) Double
Deficit Hypothesis was supported for processing speed, but not for
phonological processing. For the other group, the DDH was supported
for processing speed and phonological processing, but not for the
double deficit.
Rizza, M. G. , McIntosh, D. E., & McCunn,
A. (2001). Profile analysis of the Woodcock- Johnson III Tests of
Cognitive Abilities with gifted students. Psychology in
the Schools, 38(5), 447-455. (click to
view)
Abstract: The Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) factor
clusters of theWoodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities
(WJ III COG; Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001) were studied
with a group of gifted (n551) and nongifted individuals
(n551). Specifically, a profile analysis was
conducted using the seven CHC factors identified by the WJ III COG
authors. The gifted and nongifted individuals were between the ages
of 5 and 18 years, and were matched on gender, age, ethnicity, and
father’s level of education. Each group consisted of 23
females and 28 males. The mean age in months for the gifted group
was 135.61 (SD 5 48.81), and the mean age in months for the
nongifted group was 134.25 (SD 5 47.86). The results of the
profile analysis found gifted and nongifted individuals display
similar patterns of performance across the CHC factor clusters. As
expected, the gifted group, on average, scored consistently higher
across the set of CHC factor clusters compared to the nongifted
group. In addition, no intracognitive differences were found among
the CHC factor clusters for either the gifted or nongifted group.
Clinical and educational considerations when using the WJ III COG
when assessing giftedness are also discussed.
Rochelle, G. B. (2002). Concurrent validity of
the Wide Range Assessment of memory and learning and the Woodcock-
Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability-Revised with a
neurologically compromised pediatric
population.Dissertation Abstracts International: Section
B: The Sciences & Engineering, 62((7-B)),
3415.
Abstract: The Wide Range Assessment of Memory and
Learning (WRAML) is a relatively new instrument used in the
assessment of memory in children. The purpose of this study was to
examine the validity of the WRAML by comparing the performance of
children on both the WRAML and the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of
Cognitive Ability- Revised (WJTCA- R). Subjects for the study were
children in treatment for a brain tumor at a regional children's
medical center. Fifty children participated in the study ranging
from ages 6 to 17. A multiple regression analysis was conducted to
determine which of four selected clusters from the WJTCA-R would
have the highest correlation with the Verbal Memory Index (VERI)
from the WRAML. The Short-Term Memory (GSM) cluster had the highest
correlation (r = .82) as predicted. A Pearson's product- moment
correlational analysis was conducted between the Visual Processing
(GV) cluster from the WJTCA-R and the Visual Memory Index (VISI)
from the WRAML. GV was found to have a high positive correlation (r
= .63) with VISI. A similar analysis was conducted between the
Long- Term Retrieval (GLR) cluster from the WJTCA-R and the
Learning Index (LRNI) from the WRAML. GLR was found to have a high
positive correlation (r = .81) with LRNI. Finally, a correlational
analysis was conducted between the Broad Cognitive Ability (BCA)
scale from the WJTCA- R and the General Memory Index (GENI) from
the WRAML. A high positive correlation (r = .87) was found between
these most global measures from the two batteries. The observed
correlation between BCA and GENI was much higher than anticipated.
The author concluded that neurological impairment had affected
subject memory and intellectual functioning in similar ways. The
results do not generalize to children who have not had similar
decrements in cognitive functioning. Future research should
establish a baseline correlation between the two instruments with a
non- impaired population.
Roderiques, A. B. (2002). A comparison of
ability-achievement discrepancy models for identifying learning
disabilities.Dissertation Abstracts International Section
A: Humanities & Social Sciences, 62((8-A)),
2683.
Abstract: Three ability-achievement discrepancy
methods for identifying learning disabilities were compared. The
first method uses a simple standard score difference calculation;
the second method a regression equation; and the third, a variation
of the second, takes the standard error of estimate into account.
These three methods were examined using varying significance
criteria, producing five individual models: the simple difference
models (1A and 1B) used 16 and 23- point discrepancy criteria,
respectively; the basic regression models (2A and 2B) used 16 and
23 points; and the regression variation model (3) used a 95%
confidence level. The five models were applied to 145 students' IQ
(Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition) and
achievement (Woodcock- Johnson Achievement Tests- Revised) scores;
all students had been referred for psychoeducational testing. Mean
diagnostic proportions produced by each model yielded no
significant distinction between simple difference and basic
regression methods; however, within methods, models using less
stringent criteria identified significantly more students (p
<.05). Of the students identified by Model 1A, 17% were
declassified by Model 1B; 24% of those identified by 2A were
declassified by 2B. Model 3 functioned much like Model 2B.
Students' classification across models was dependent on their age
and ability level. These findings suggest that the criterion chosen
for significance has more impact on eligibility outcomes than the
discrepancy method.
Ronning,
M. E. (2004). Core profile types for the Cognitive Assessment
System and Woodcock- Johnson Tests of Achievement-
Revised. Dissertation Abstracts International,
65(3- A), 869.
Abstract: The present study was conducted in two
phases. Phase 1 involved the development of ability/achievement
normative taxonomies for reading and mathematics using the
multivariate techniques of cluster analysis. The core profiles that
emerged provide important comparisons for evaluating individual
profiles, as well as add to the information explaining common
variability in the child population. The taxonomies were based upon
711 children in the 8 to 17 year old portion of the standardization
sample of the Cognitive Assessment System (CAS) who were co-
administered the Woodcock- Johnson Tests of Achievement- Revised
(WJ-R ACH). Ability/reading and ability/math normative taxonomies
were developed from the Planning, Attention, Simultaneous, and
Successive scales of the CAS in conjunction with four reading and
three math WJ-R ACH subscales. Eight reading and five math clusters
were identified and described using demographics and overall
ability and achievement levels. In Phase 2, the prevalence of
students with low reading and math achievement in each cluster was
examined. Ramifications for intervention planning are
discussed.
Short, E. J., Klein, N. K., Lewis, B. A.,
Fulton, S., Eisengart, S., Kercsmar, C., Baley, J., & Singer,
L. T. (2003). Cognitive and Academic Consequences of
Bronchopulmonary Dysplasia and Very Low Birth Weight: 8- Year-Old
Outcomes.Pediatrics, 112(5), 359-
366.(click to
view)
Abstract: To examine the effects of bronchopulmonary
dysplasia (BPD) and very low birth weight (VLBW) on the cognitive
and academic achievement of a large sample of 8- year- old
children. METHODS: Infants who were VLBW and had BPD (n = 98) or
did not have BPD (n = 75) and term infants (n = 99) were followed
prospectively to age 8. Groups were compared on measures assessing
4 broad areas of functioning: intelligence, achievement, gross
motor, and attentional skills. Measures included the Wechsler
Intelligence Scale for Children III, the Woodcock Johnson Test of
Achievement- Revised, the Bruininks- Oseretsky Test of
Motor Proficiency, the Tactual Performance Test (spatial memory),
and the Continuous Performance Test (attention). School outcomes
were assessed by parent and teacher report, as well as from school
records. Groups were comparable on socioeconomic status, sex, and
race. The total sample of BPD, VLBW, and term children was compared
on all outcome measures. In addition, neurologic risk was assessed
in the present sample and included the following: intraventricular
hemorrhage, echodense lesions, porencephaly, hydrocephalus,
ventriculoperitoneal shunt, meningitis, and periventricular
leukomalacia. Individual difference analyses were conducted for
neurologically intact children in all 3 groups. Finally, treatment
effects were examined by comparing BPD children who had received
steroids as part of their treatment with BPD children who had
not.
Skinner,
C. L. (2001). Describing the cognitive, socioemotional, and
academic sequelae of children with acquired brain
injury.Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B:
The Sciences & Engineering, 62((2- B)),
1137.
Abstract: The purpose of this study was to examine the
effects of acquired brain injury (ABI) using the Das- Naglieri
Cognitive Assessment System (CAS), the Parent Rating Scale of the
Behavior Assessment System for Children (BASC), and the Woodcock-
Johnson Tests of Achievement (WJ- R). In particular, this study
explored how deficits in planning, attention, simultaneous and
successive processing may impact a student with ABI socially,
emotionally, and academically. Participants in the study included
27 children and adolescents who had received an acquired brain
injury. These injuries occurred less than one year to 13 years
prior to testing, with an mean of 5.1 years. This sample obtained
significantly lower means than the standardization sample on the
Planning, Attention, Simultaneous, and Successive scales of the
CAS. Additionally, this sample received significantly lower means
than the standardization sample on the Broad Reading and Broad Math
scales of the WJ-R, and on the Externalizing Composite,
Internalizing Composite, and Behavioral Symptoms Index of the BASC.
Positive relationships were demonstrated between the CAS and the
WJ-R. Because high scores on the CAS indicate greater cognitive
ability and low scores on the BASC indicate no socioemotional
problems, it was assumed that any relationship between the CAS and
the BASC would be negative. This study failed to reveal any
significant negative relationships between these measures. This
study demonstrated the ability of the CAS in detecting deficits in
planning, attention, successive, and simultaneous processing.
Moreover, significant deficits were found in mathematics and
reading using the WJ-R. This study also demonstrated the ability of
the BASC to detect socioemotional problems. The CAS, WJ-R, and BASC
all appear to be useful and valid instruments for examining the
cognitive, academic, and socioemotional effects of acquired brain
injury.
Suarez, N. H. (2005). The effect of a memory
intervention on short-term memory with children diagnosed with mild
neurological deficits. Dissertation Abstracts
International Section A: Humanities & Social Sciences,
65(7- A), 2493.
Abstract: This study investigated the effects of a
memory intervention on the short-term memory of children with mild
neurological impairments. Nine children, ages 4 to 9 participated.
The research considered the core of memory function itself, based
on an information processing model of cognition and memory. This
intervention approach was more diverse than found in previous
studies. Rather than the single modality restriction, three
modalities (visual, auditory, and kinesthetic) were considered. The
intervention involved practice repeating randomized notes played on
a 4-note Little Tykes piano, and then imitated by the participant.
A multiple baseline design across individuals was used to show
improvement in short- and long- term ordered memory after
intervention implementation. Experimental effects were determined
by introducing the intervention to three groups of children, at
different points in time after baseline stabilization, resulting in
behavior changes during intervention periods and not at any prior
time. Effects were shown in a controlled fashion for 7 of the 9
participants, showing increases in mean scores and upward trends
during intervention. Second, the effects of the memory intervention
on Stanford- Binet IV pre and post Test Composite scores were
examined. Scores increased for all 9 participants, with a range of
1 to 18 standard score points, and a mean increase of 9.77 points.
Intervention effects were also compared using pre- and post-testing
on the Visual Auditory Learning subtest from the Woodcock Johnson
III, multiple measures of Bead Memory and Memory for Sentences from
the Stanford- Binet IV, and Memory for Words from the Woodcock
Johnson III. Visual Auditory Learning subtest scores increased from
4 to 18 points, with a mean increase of 11 points. Multiple
measures of Bead Memory, Memory for Words, and Memory for Sentences
in general showed increased standard scores greater than the SEm
for those subtests, however, increases were not consistent per
participant. A positive correlation of +.5737 was found between
increases in notes played and differences in SB-IV Test Composite
pre-post scores. For a sample size of 9, a significance of .10 was
not reached.
Viljoen,
J. , & Roesch, R. (2005). Competence to Waive
Interrogation Rights and Adjudicative Competence in Adolescent
Defendants: Cognitive Development, Attorney Contact, and
Psychological Symptoms. Law and Human Behavior,
29( 6), 723- 742.
Abstract: Although there is growing evidence of
developmental differences in competency to waive interrogation
rights and adjudicative competence, the correlates of adolescents'
legal capacities remain unclear. This study examined the
relationship of legal capacities to cognitive development, legal
learning opportunities, and psychological symptoms. Participants
were 152 male and female defendants aged 11–17, who completed
Grisso's Instruments for Assessing Understanding and Appreciation
of Miranda Rights, the Fitness Interview Test (Revised Edition),
the Woodcock- Johnson III Cognitive Assessment Battery, and the
Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale for Children. Legal capacities
relevant to interrogation and adjudication increased with age.
These developmental differences were partially mediated or
explained by cognitive development. Of the specific cognitive
ilities examined (general intellectual ability, verbal ability,
reasoning, long- term retrieval, attention, and executive
functioning), verbal ability was a particularly strong predictor of
performance on competency measures. Also, defendants obtained lower
scores on competency measures if they showed evidence of attention
deficits or hyperactivity, had spent limited time with their
attorneys, and/or were from low socioeconomic
backgrounds.
Wells, J. W. (2005). Exploring relative cognitive
strengths among rigorously selected private school students with
ADHD. Dissertation Abstracts International,
66(7), 3964.
Abstract: This study is designed to explore cognitive
strengths and weaknesses, as assessed with the Woodcock Johnson-
Revised Cognitive Battery, of a sample of rigorously selected
private school students with ADHD. ADHD is a disorder that can
significantly impact cognitive functioning and academic
performance. Research strongly indicates deficits in auditory
processing, processing speed for individuals with ADHD. Based on
research on gifted students with learning disabilities,
academically successful students with ADHD may rely on cognitive
strengths to compensate for cognitive weaknesses associated with
the disorder. It was hypothesized that the sample would yield
significant cognitive strengths as well as cognitive weaknesses
characteristically associated with ADHD. For the purpose of this
study, a Standard Deviation- Difference score of +1 was considered
strength whereas a Standard Deviation- Difference score of -
1 was considered weakness. A sample of 72 students with ADHD
referred for psycho- educational testing from an academically
rigorous private school was used for this study. A 7 (broad
cognitive ability) by 3 (relative performance: strength, weakness,
or neither) crosstabulation was constructed to examine the joint
probability of these events. In addition, a 2 (sample: norm versus
study) by 3 (relative performance: strength, weakness, neither)
chi-square test was conducted for the seven cognitive abilities.
Results of this study indicate that fluid reasoning, auditory
processing, processing speed, short-term retrieval, and long- term
retrieval are significantly different from the norm sample at p
<.05. Magnitude of difference was calculated by coefficient
of contingency (C). Fluid reasoning C = .35) and short-term
memory (C = .37) appear to be serve as cognitive strengths
whereas auditory processing (C = .63) and long- term
retrieval (C = .45) appear to be cognitive weaknesses for these
students with ADHD. Processing speed (C = .3) split evenly as
strength and weakness. This study demonstrates that these students
do have significant cognitive strengths as well as the
characteristic weaknesses of students with ADHD. These results have
clinical implications for the treatment of ADHD in the academic
environment.
|
|