Bold font designates new information not in
prior version.
Allen, K. D. (2005). Metacognitive systematic
inquiry utilizing individualized cognitive profiles causes reading
comprehension achievement. Dissertation Abstracts
International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences.,
66(4-A), 1271.
Abstract: The positive effects of systematic
metacognitive instruction on reading achievement have been
demonstrated, but that research has generally not been translated
into classroom practice. This mixed methods research study sought
to facilitate reading comprehension by involving subjects
metacognitively with profiles of their own cognitive strengths and
weaknesses. The study was conducted with 196 intermediate
elementary students in the naturalized setting of 10 classrooms.
Student scores from Woodcock- Johnson III cognitive clusters were
utilized to generate individual cognitive profiles. In each
classroom there were three experimental levels: (1) cognitive
assessment only (control group) vs. (2) cognitive assessment +
profile awareness (profile awareness group) vs. (3) cognitive
assessment + cognitive profile awareness + metacognitive systematic
inquiry (metacognitive systematic inquiry group). The metacognitive
systematic inquiry treatment occurred as part of classroom
independent reading instruction with judgments of learning,
feedback, self- reflection, and comprehension questions related to
those individual cognitive strengths and weaknesses which have been
shown to correlate with reading comprehension. This treatment
yielded significantly higher comprehension on a state standardized
reading test, but not on an informal reading inventory. In a
qualitative analysis, the treatment groups seemed to be more
proficient at articulating declarative knowledge about individual
cognitive abilities and reading strengths, as well as procedural
knowledge about the connection between reading comprehension and
cognitive ability. This study provides an example of how research
findings in metacognition and metacomprehension can be generalized
into classroom practice.
Baker, L., Mackler, K., Sonnenschein, S.,
&Serpell, R. (2001). Parents' interactions with their first-
grade children during storybook reading and relations with
subsequent home reading activity and reading achievement.
Journal of School Psychology, 39 (5), 415-
438.(click to
view)
Abstract: This study examined parents' verbal
and affective interactions with their first-grade children during
shared storybook reading and how these interactions relate to
growth in children's reading activity and achievement. Participants
varied in income level and ethnicity. The nature and amount of
meaning-related talk was similar regardless of whether the parent
or child assumed primary responsibility for reading, but there was
more talk about the reading process itself (word recognition) when
the child read. Talk that went beyond the immediate content of the
story was more common among middle-income families. Positive
affective interactions were associated with meaning-related talk,
and negative interactions were associated with parental attempts to
have the child use decoding strategies to identify unknown words.
Affective quality was an important contributor to children's
reading of challenging materials in third grade but not to their
reading achievement. Implications for advising parents on reading
with their children are considered.
Buckhalt, J. & El-Sheikh,
M. Children’s Sleep and Cognitive Functioning: Race and
Socioeconomic Status as Moderators of
Effects. Child Development, 78(1),
213–231.(Click to
view).
Race and socioeconomic status (SES) moderated the link
between children’s sleep and cognitive functioning.One
hundred and sixty-six 8- to 9-year-old African and European
American children varying in SES participated. Sleep
measures were actigraphy, sleep diaries, and self-report; cognitive
measures were from the Woodcock – Johnson III and
reaction time tasks. Children had similar performance when sleep
was more optimal, but after controlling for SES, African
American children had lower performance with sleep
disruptions.Children from lower and higher SES had similar
performance with better sleep quality and less variability insleep
schedules, but when sleep was more disrupted, higher SES children
had better performance. Examination of environmental
variables associated with race and SES that may underlie these
effects may lead to directions for interventions to improve
cognitive performance.
De Von Figueroa-Moseley, C., Ramey, C.
T., Keltner, B., & Lanzi, R.
G. (2006). Variations in Latino Parenting Practices and
Their Effects on Child Cognitive Developmental
Outcomes. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences,
28(1), 102-114. (click to
view)
Abstract: This research examines variations in
parenting and its effects on child cognitive outcomes across Latino
subgroups from a national sampling that utilized a subset of 995
former Head Start Latino parents and children. Comparisons of the
Parenting Dimension Inventory scaled scores revealed Latino
subgroup differences on nurturance and consistency. Puerto Rican
parental caregivers scored higher on nurturance and consistency
than Mexican Americans and El Salvadorans. Pearson’s
product-moment correlation revealed a positive relationship between
responsiveness to child inputs and higher academic achievement
scores for each Latino subgroup. Examination of the
Woodcock-Johnson Test of Achievement also revealed a main effect of
Latino subgroup differences. Findings suggest that there exist
intracultural variations in parenting and academic achievement
within the Latino population and confirm previous findings that
parenting is related to positive child cognitive
outcomes.
Floyd, R., Bergeron, R., McCormack, A.,
Anderson, J., Hargrove-Owens, G. (2005). Are
Cattell-Horn-Carroll Broad Ability Composite Scores Exchangeable
Across Batteries? School Psychology Review, 34(3),
329-357. (click to
view)
Abstract. Many school
psychologists use the Cattell-Hom-Carroll (CHC) theory of cognitive
abilities to guide their interpretation of scores from intelligence
test batteries. Some may frequently assume that composite scores
purported to measure the same CHC broad abilities should be
relatively similar for individuals no matter what subtests or
batteries were administered to obtain these scores. This study
examined this assumption using six samples of preschool children,
school- age children, or adults who completed two or more
intelligence test batteries. From these samples, composites
measuring the broad abilities Crystallized Intelligence, Visual
Processing, Fluid Reasoning, and Processing Speed were compared to
examine their exchangeability. Results indicate that most CHC broad
ability composites produced scores that were not as exchangeable
for individuals as may have been assumed by some. Discussion
focuses on the influence of score reliability and on the
interaction between examinee characteristics and the tasks used to
measure the broad abilities.
Frisby, C. & Osterlind, S.
(2006). A Descriptive Analysis of Test Session Observation
Checklist Ratings From the Woodcock Johnson III Standardization
Sample,Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 24
(4), 342-257. (click
to view)
Abstract. Modern scale construction
techniques have been used to develop scales measuring examiner
ratings of examinees’ test session behavior (TSB) on Wechsler
and Stanford-Binet intelligence tests. This study analyzes data
from the Test Session Observation Checklist (TSOC), a measure
developed by post hoc rational analysis, from a portion of the
Woodcock Johnson Psychoeducational Battery–Third Edition
standardization sample (N = 5,769). Results
indicate (a) most participants are rated as displaying minimal
negative test session behaviors, (b) the seven TSOC items are
organized into two components (General Impressions and Attention To
Task) that explain roughly 54% of the variance in TSOC scores, and
(c) race, sex, and age differences in component scores examined
with and without covariates are consistent with results from prior
TSB research.
Frisby,
C. & Osterlind, S. (2007b). Hispanic Test-Session
Behavior on the Woodcock Johnson Psychoeducational
Battery– Third Edition.Journal of
Psyhoeducational Assessment, 25 (3), 257- 270.
(click
to view)
Abstract: This study examined
potential differential examiner ratings for a large sample of
self-identified Hispanics on the Woodcock Johnson Psychoeducational
Battery–Third Edition (WJ-III) Test Session Observation
Checklist (TSOC). Both between- group (Hispanics vs. non-Hispanics)
and within-group analyses (Hispanics disaggregated by first spoken
language, language spoken in the home, and mother’s highest
educational level) were conducted. Four research hypotheses were
tested through 44 analyses. Most comparisons were not statistically
significant, and across- and within-group differences had minimal
influence in analyses that were statistically significant. The
authors conclude that there is no compelling evidence of
substantial systematic differences in examiner ratings of
Hispanics’ test-session behaviors on the
WJ-III.
Krasa, N. (2007) s the
Woodcock-Johnson III a Test for All Seasons? Ceiling and Item
Gradient Considerations in Its Use With Older
Students. Journal of Psychoeducational
Assessment, 25(1), 3- 16. (click to
view).
This study assesses the adequacy of item gradients and
ceilings for the subtests of the Woodcock-Johnson III (WJ III)
Cognitive and Achievement batteries, including the Diagnostic
Supplement, in their use with participants ages 16 to 25 and Grades
10 to 18. Of the 52 subtests, 18 contain adequate item gradients
and ceilings for the entire age and grade range. The remaining 34
subtests have inadequate ceilings and/or inadequate item gradients
in at least the interval between the first and second standard
deviations above the mean, as predicted by the growth curves of the
factor clusters, which peak around age 25. The report discusses
developmental and practical implications and suggests improvements
for the next revision of the WJ.
Gormley,
W. T., Gayer, T., Phillips, D., & Dawson, B. (2005). The
Effects of Universal Pre-K on Cognitive Development.
Developmental Psychology, 41( 6), 872-884.
(click to
view)
Abstract: In this study of Oklahoma's universal pre-K
program, the authors relied on a strict birthday eligibility
criterion to compare "young" kindergarten children who just
completed pre- K to "old" pre-K children just beginning pre-K. This
regression- discontinuity design reduces the threat of selection
bias. Their sample consisted of 1,567 pre- K children and 1,461
kindergarten children who had just completed pre- K. The authors
estimated the impact of the pre-K treatment on Woodcock- Johnson
Achievement test scores. The authors found test impacts of 3.00
points (0.79 of the standard deviation for the control group) for
the Letter- Word Identification score, 1.86 points (0.64 of the
standard deviation of the control group) for the Spelling score,
and 1.94 points (0.38 of the standard deviation of the control
group) for the Applied Problems score. Hispanic, Black, White, and
Native American children all benefit from the program, as do
children in diverse income brackets, as measured by school lunch
eligibility status. The authors conclude that Oklahoma's universal
pre-K program has succeeded in enhancing the school readiness of a
diverse group of children.
Haley, J.
A. (2002). The relationship between instrumental music
instruction and academic achievement in fourth grade students.
Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: Humanities
& Social Sciences, 62((9-A)), 2696.
Abstract: The purpose of this research is to
investigate the relationship between academic achievement in
elementary school children participating in an instrumental music
program (band or orchestra) to those children not participating in
an instrumental music program. While research in this area has
suggested a positive correlation between exposure to music and
cognitive abilities (spatial, temporal, mathematical, language
abilities), it is yet to be determined if learning to play a
musical instrument is related to academic achievement. Seventy,
fourth grade children enrolled in regular classroom settings were
evaluated in spelling achievement, arithmetic achievement and
phonemic awareness in the beginning of the school year (prior to
the introduction of instrumental instruction) and reevaluated nine
months later, at the end of the school year. Additionally, a short-
questionnaire was given to each child. All participants were from a
suburban, public elementary school. The comparison groups are:
children studying an instrument prior to the introduction of band
and orchestra in fourth grade (Group A), children just beginning
the study of an instrument (Group B), and children with no
experience in instrumental instruction (Group C). For all three
groups, the academic achievement scores obtained in the beginning
of the year were compared to the academic achievement scores
obtained at the end of the school year. Spelling and mathematical
achievement were assessed using the Wide Range Achievement
Test-III. Tests of phonemic awareness were administered using the
Word Identification and Word Attack subtests from the Woodcock
Johnson Test of Achievement. While both the experimental groups and
the control group were expected to show gains after one year across
all dependent variables, it was hypothesized that those children
participating in an instrumental music program would show
significantly greater gains: (a) on a test of spelling achievement,
(b) on a test of mathematical achievement, and (c) on a test of
word identification, and (d) on a test of phonemic awareness, than
those children not participating in an instrumental program. It was
further hypothesized that those children studying an instrument for
greater than one school year would demonstrate greater gains across
spelling, math, word identification, and phonemic awareness than
those children with only one year of experience or no
experience.
Mccreith, T. M. (2005). A construct
comparability analysis of cognitive ability tests in different
languages.Dissertation Abstracts International Section A:
Humanities & Social Sciences, 65(8-A),
2962.
Abstract: This research studies the construct
comparability of the Woodcock-Johnson Battery - Third Edition Tests
of Cognitive Ability (WJ III COG; Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather,
2001) and the BaterIa III Woodcock- Munoz: Pruebas de Habilidad
Cognitiva - Third Edition (BaterIa III COG; Woodcock, Munoz-
Sandoval, McGrew, Mather, & Schrank, in press- b), which are
the English and Spanish versions of the same battery, respectively.
These are measures of cognitive functioning that purport to be
direct counterparts of one another. This study examined the degree
of comparability and sources of incomparability of seven tests of
cognitive ability that were translated from English to Spanish. The
purpose of this study was to determine: (1) whether the
dimensionality and structure of each of the selected tests of the
WJ III COG and BaterIa III COG were the same; (2) whether there
were specific items from the selected tests of the WJ III COG and
BaterIa III COG that function differentially for English- and
Spanish- speaking examinees; and (3) whether the sources of
differences in constructs being assessed for the two language
groups could be identified. Answers to the research questions
stated above contributed to evidence relevant for determining the
comparability of the inferences based on these test scores for two
different language versions. Between the two language versions of
the tests, at the scale as well as the item level, the results
indicated that there were different levels of psychometric
similarities and differences for some of the seven tests that may
jeopardize the comparability of scores from these
versions.
Ofiesh,
N., Mather, N., & Russell, A. (2005). Using speeded cognitive,
reading, and academic measures to determine the need for extended
test time among university students with, learning
disabilities. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment,
23( 1), 35- 52. (click to
view)
Abstract: This study examined the relationship
between scores on “speeded” cognitive and academic
tests and the need for the accommodation of extended test time for
normally achieving students (NA) and students with learning
disabilities (LD). Often, in postsecondary settings the decision to
provide the accommodation of extended test time is based largely on
the diagnostic test scores in the student's LD documentation.
Therefore, the primary purpose of this study was to evaluate the
relationship between specific diagnostic tests and the need for the
accommodation of extended test time. A secondary purpose was to
investigate the relationships and predictive ability of five
speeded cognitive tests, three speeded cluster scores, and two
measures of timed reading. Correlations and logistic regression
analyses were used to assess gain in score performance and predict
the need for extended test time. Participants included 41 NA
university students and 43 university students with LD. The
findings indicated significant group differences on all speeded
cognitive, reading, and academic tests, with the exception of Digit
Symbol on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III and Retrieval
Fluency and Decision Speed tests on the Woodcock- Johnson III Tests
of Cognitive Abilities. The Reading Fluency test and the Academic
Fluency cluster of the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement III
were the best predictors of students with LD who needed extended
time on the multiple- choice reading comprehension
test.
Patel, B.
N. , Seltzer, G. B., Wu, H., & Schupf, N. (2001). Effect of
menopause on cognitive performance in women with Down syndrome.
Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience
Research, 12(12), 2659- 2662.
Abstract: Compared cognitive function in nondemented
pre- and postmenopausal females with Down's syndrome. Ss (aged 21-
57 yrs) comprised 58 premenopausal females (mean age 34.7 yrs) and
24 postmenopausal females (mean age 49.7 yrs). Ss were assessed for
cognitive function using the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive
Ability-- Revised (WJTCA- R) at baseline and at 2- yr follow-up.
Results show that premenopausal Ss performed better than
age-matched male controls, while postmenopausal Ss performed more
poorly than age- matched male controls. Premenopausal females and
young male controls showed no significant declines in cognition at
2- yr follow-up. Postmenopausal Ss, but not age-matched male
controls, showed significant declines in cognitive function at 2-yr
follow-up. It is concluded that cognitive function declines in
postmenopausal females are associated with estrogen deficiency
rather than with age
Rizza, M. G., McIntosh, D. E., & McCunn,
A. (2001). Profile analysis of the Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of
Cognitive Abilities with gifted students. Psychology in
the Schools, 38(5),
447-455.(click
to view)
Abstract: The Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) factor
clusters of theWoodcock- Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities
(WJ III COG; Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001) were studied
with a group of gifted (n551) and nongifted individuals
(n551). Specifically, a profile analysis was
conducted using the seven CHC factors identified by the WJ III COG
authors. The gifted and nongifted individuals were between the ages
of 5 and 18 years, and were matched on gender, age, ethnicity, and
father’s level of education. Each group consisted of 23
females and 28 males. The mean age in months for the gifted group
was 135.61 (SD 5 48.81), and the mean age in months for the
nongifted group was 134.25 (SD 5 47.86). The results of the
profile analysis found gifted and nongifted individuals display
similar patterns of performance across the CHC factor clusters. As
expected, the gifted group, on average, scored consistently higher
across the set of CHC factor clusters compared to the nongifted
group. In addition, no intracognitive differences were found among
the CHC factor clusters for either the gifted or nongifted group.
Clinical and educational considerations when using the WJ III COG
when assessing giftedness are also discussed.
Sanborn, K. J., Truscott, S. D., Phelps, L.,
&McDougal, J. L. (2003). Does the Flynn Effect differ by IQ
level in samples of students classified as learning
disabled?Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment,
21(2), 145- 159. (click to
view)
Abstract: This research examined scores from two
learning-disabled (LD) samples for evidence of the Flynn Effect
(FE; Flynn, 1999) to determine (a) whether the FE was evident, and
(b) if the magnitude of the FE differed on IQ scores at varying
levels of intelligence. Sample 1 consisted of 40 children who were
administered the Woodcock- Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability-
Revised (WJ-R Cog) and the Woodcock- Johnson Tests of Cognitive
Ability-Ill (WJ- III Cog) approximately 8 weeks apart. Sample 2
consisted of triennial test data for 169 students who were tested
once with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-
Revised (WISC-R) and 3 years later with the Wechsler
Intelligence Scale for Children- Third Edition (WISC- III).
After an initial analysis for the overall FE, Samples 1 and 2 were
each divided into IQ level groups (e.g., 91-105) and analyzed
accordingly. Results for Sample 1 (W-J) indicated that the 2+ point
observed IQ change was not significant for this relatively small
sample, although it appeared to be consistent with the direction
and degree predicted by the FE (Flynn, 1984). Results for Sample 2
(WISC) indicated a significant IQ change consistent with the FE.
Neither sample exhibited statistically significant differences for
FE by IQ level. However, consistent with previous research,
observed differences increased from lower to higher IQ levels.
Consequently, this research cannot rule out the possibility that a
child's IQ level influences the degree to which the FE is
apparent.
Simos, P., Fletcher, J., Sarkari, S.,
Billingsley-Marshall, R., Denton, C. & Papanicolaou, C.
(2007). Intensive Instruction Affects Brain Magnetic
Activity Associated with Oral Word Reading in Children with
Persistent Reading Disabilities. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 40(1), 37-
48.(click to
view).
Abstract: Fifteen children ages 7 to 9
years who had persistent reading difficulties despite adequate
instruction were provided with intensive tutorial interventions.
The interventions targeted deficient phonological processing and
decoding skills for 8 weeks (2 hours per day) followed by an
8-week, 1-hour-per-day intervention that focused on the development
of reading fluency skills. Spatiotemporal brain activation profiles
were obtained at baseline and after each 8-week intervention
program using magnetoencephalography during the performance of an
oral sight- word reading task. Changes in brain activity were found
in the posterior part of the middle temporal gyrus (Brodmann's Area
[BA] 21: increased degree of activity and reduced onset latency),
the lateral occipitotemporal region (BA 19/37: decreased onset
latency of activation), and the premotor cortex (increased onset
latency). Overall changes associated with the intervention were
primarily normalizing, as indicated by (a) increased activity in a
region that is typically involved in lexical- semantic processing
(BA 21) and (b) a shift in the relative timing of regional activity
in temporal and frontal cortices to a pattern typically seen in
unimpaired readers. These findings extend previous results in
demonstrating significant changes in the spatiotemporal profile of
activation associated with word reading in response to reading
remediation.
Taub, G.,
McGrew, K. &Keith, T. (2007). Improvements in interval
time tracking and effects on reading achievement. Psychology in the
Schools, 44(8), 849-863. (click to
view)
Abstract: This study
examined the effect of improvements in timing/rhythmicity on
students’ reading achievement. 86 participants completed pre-
and post-test measures of reading achievement (i.e., Woodcock-
Johnson III, Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing, Test of
Word Reading Efficiency, and Test of Silent Word Reading Fluency).
Students in the experimental group completed a 4- week intervention
designed to improve their timing/rhythmicity by reducing the
latency in their response to a synchronized metronome beat,
referred to as a synchronized metronome tapping (SMT) intervention.
The results from this non-
academicintervention indicate the experimental
group’s post-test scores on select measures of reading were
significantly higher than the non-treatment control group’s
scores at the end of 4 weeks. This paper provides a brief overview
of domain-general cognitive abilities believed effected by SMT
interventions and provides a preliminary hypothesis to explain how
thisnon- academic intervention can demonstrate
a statistically significant effect on students’ reading
achievement scores.
Tornquist,
E. H. (2005). The relationship between poverty and disability for
special education students.Dissertation Abstracts
International, 66(2), 552.
Abstract: Recently, negative attention has focused on
the disproportionate increase in special education enrollment.
Particular attention has been given to the overrepresentation of
minority students identified as students with disabilities.
Research has suggested that the relationships between poverty and
disability may partially explain this disproportionality. The
purpose of this study was to examine the effects of poverty and
poverty measures on elementary special education students. This
study used data from the first wave of the Special Education
Longitudinal Study (SEELS) to describe student and household
characteristics and to identify the extent to which income and
poverty are associated with academic achievement and disability
using multiple poverty measures (Wagner, Marder, Blackorby,
& Cardosa, 2004). The SEELS data were obtained from parent
interviews and from student assessments based on subtests of the
Woodcock Johnson. The data set consisted of 9,747 parent interviews
and 4,912 student assessments. Since the federal poverty measure is
widely agreed to be an inaccurate measure, multiple definitions of
poverty were employed in the present study including: 50% of the
federal threshold, the federal threshold, 150% and 200% of the
federal threshold. Across all levels of poverty and non-poverty,
descriptive findings indicated that in special education there are
more boys than girls, the majority speak English at home, ethnicity
differs by poverty category, and that the percentages of students
living with both parents increase as income increases. Descriptive
findings also indicate that: poorer students are more likely to
live in households with more people, lower income families are more
likely to have lower levels of parental educational attainment, and
that students from lower income families are more likely to have
parents that were never married. Findings of interest were
uncovered in the extreme poverty category with more pronounced
differences revealed in gender and ethnicity. Regression analyses
indicated that poverty is a significant predictor of passage
comprehension scores and calculation scores. Overall, this research
suggests that poverty is a more significant predictor for academic
achievement than ethnicity and that the lower a student's income,
the less positive their circumstances are. Findings are discussed
with respect to future research, policy and
practice.
VanNoord, R. G., & Prevatt, F. F. (2002).
Rater agreement on IQ and achievement tests effect on evaluations
of learning disabilities. Journal of School Psychology,
40(2), 167-176. (click to
view)
Abstract: Protocols from 110 evaluations
utilizing the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children- Third
Edition (WISC- III) and the Woodcock/Johnson Tests of Achievement-
Revised (W/J-R) were scored by two different raters to determine
(a) whether subtests with more difficult levels of scoring yield
lower interrater correlation coefficients, (b) whether scoring
errors on subtests affect broad score estimates, (c) the effect of
expertise of rater on scoring errors, and (d) whether scoring
errors affect a learning disability determination based on
IQ/achievement discrepancy. Scoring errors were found on almost 25%
of Comprehension and Vocabulary subtests; however, the effect of
these scoring errors was minimal. About 42% of Writing Samples
subtests had scoring errors, resulting in a mean change of 1.75
points on the Broad Written Language Cluster subtest. On the
WISC-III, but not the W/J- R, there were significantly more errors
made by inexperienced testers. Scoring errors resulted in two cases
in which learning disability determination would be changed.
Overall, the study corroborates previous findings of strong
interrater reliability on most subtests of common IQ and
achievement tests and indicates that novice scorers are not likely
to make scoring mistakes that will significantly impact an
IQ/achievement discrepancy- based documentation of learning
disability.
Waschbusch, D. A., Daleiden, E., & Drabman, R. S. (2000).
Are parents accurate reporters of their child's cognitive
abilities? Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral
Assessment, 22(1), 61- 77.
|
|