Bold font designates new information not in
prior version.
Ferrer, E., & McArdle, J. J. (2004). An
experimental analysis of dynamic hypotheses about cognitive
abilities and achievement from childhood to early
adulthood.Developmental
Psychology,40(6), 935-
952.(click to
view)
Abstract: This study examined the dynamics of
cognitive abilities and academic achievement from childhood to
early adulthood. Predictions about time-dependent "coupling"
relations between cognition and achievement based on R. B.
Cattell's (1971, 1987) investment hypothesis were evaluated using
linear dynamic models applied to longitudinal data (N=672).
Contrary to Cattell's hypothesis, a first set of findings indicated
that fluid and crystallized abilities, as defined by the
Woodcock-Johnson Psycho- Educational Battery-Revised (WJ-R; R. W.
Woodcock & M. B. Johnson, 1989-1990), were not dynamically
coupled with each other over time. A second set of findings
provided support for the original predictions and indicated that
fluid ability was a leading indicator of changes in achievement
measures (i.e., quantitative ability and general academic
knowledge). The findings of this study suggest that the dynamics of
cognitive abilities and academic achievement follow a more complex
pattern than that specified by Cattell's investment
hypothesis.
Ferrer, E., Salthouse, T. A., McArdle, J. J.,
Stewart, W. F., & Schwartz, B. S. (2005). Multivariate
modeling of age and retest in longitudinal studies of cognitive
abilities.Psychology and Aging, 20(3), 412-
422.(click to
view)
Abstract: Longitudinal multivariate mixed models
were used to examine the correlates of change between memory and
processing speed and the contribution of age and retest to such
change correlates. Various age- and occasion-mixed models were
fitted to 2 longitudinal data sets of adult individuals (N
>1,200). For both data sets, the results indicated that the
correlation between the age slopes of memory and processing speed
decreased when retest effects were included in the model. If retest
effects existed in the data but were not modeled, the correlation
between the age slopes was positively biased. The authors suggest
that although the changes in memory and processing speed may be
correlated over time, age alone does not capture such a
covariation.
Kail, R. & Ferrer, E. (2007).
Processing Speed in Childhood and Adolescence: Longitudinal
Models for Examining Developmental
Change. Child Development, 78
(6), 1760 – 1770.(click to
view)
The
primary aim of the present study was to examine longitudinal models
to determine the function that best describes developmental change
in processing speed during childhood and adolescence. In one
sample, children and adolescents (N 5503) were tested twice over an
average interval of 2 years on two psychometric measures of
processing speed: Visual Matching and Cross Out. In another sample,
children and adolescents (N 5 277) were tested four times, every 6
months, on Cross Out. Age-related changes in performance on both
tasks were examined using six longitudinal models representing
different hypotheses of growth. Linear, hyperbolic, inverse
regression, and transition models yielded relatively poor fit to
the data; the fit of the exponential and quadratic models was
substantially better. The heuristic value of these latter models is
discussed.
Litke, D.
R. (2001). Implicit learning and development .
Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences
& Engineering, 61((11-B)), 6159.
Abstract: The present study evaluated Reber's (1992)
claim that compared to explicit learning (EL), implicit learning
(IL) will be less affected by development. Sixty subjects, 20 in
each of 3 age groups (2nd- grade, 6th- grade, and
college), were measured on explicit and implicit tasks. The
Analysis- Synthesis subtest of the Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-
Educational Battery-Revised (1989) was used to measure EL. A
modified version of the Serial Reaction Time task developed by
Nissen and Bullemer (1987) was used to measure IL. Results showed
that while performance on the explicit task increased with grade,
performance on the implicit task, as predicted, showed no
significant differences across different grades. These results
support Reber's claim of the relative age- independence of IL.
Furthermore, subjects' performances on EL and IL tasks were
uncorrelated, suggesting that EL and IL abilities may reflect
different cognitive systems. These findings provide indirect
support for Reber's theory that implicit and explicit cognitive
systems can be differentiated by the period of evolution in which
they are thought to have emerged.
Mattison, R. E., Hooper, S. R., &
Glassberg, L. A. (2002). Three-year course of learning disorders in
special education students classified as behavioral
disorder.Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(12), 1454-1461.(click to
view)
Abstract: Objective: To investigate the 3-year
course of learning disorders (LDs) and academic achievement in a
sample of students with psychiatric disorders who were newly
classified by the special education category of behavioral disorder
(BD). Method: The occurrence of four definitions for LD (both
discrepancy and low achievement) based on the WISC-R and the
Woodcock-Johnson Psychoeducational Battery was followed in 81
students with BD from the time of their enrollment in BD classes to
their first reevaluation after 3 years. Odds ratios (ORs) were used
to measure stability of LDs in these students. Results: The
prevalence of any LD was 64.2% at baseline and 61.7% at follow-up.
Most of the 10 possible LD categories showed significant ORs, and
the average OR was 21.9. At follow- up after 3 years, students both
with and without LD at baseline had approximately the same
achievement standard scores in reading and mathematics, but a
significantly lower score for written language. Standard scores for
the students without LD consistently were significantly higher than
the scores for students with comorbid LD. Conclusion: LDs in this
unique sample of students with psychiatric disorders remained
common and generally stable over the first 3 years.
McArdle, J. J., & Woodcock, R. W. (1997).
Expanding test-retest designs to include developmental time- lag
components. Psychological Methods, 2(4),
403- 435.(click to
view)
Abstract. Test-retest data can reflect
systematic changes over varying intervals of time in a "time- lag"
design. This article shows how latent growth models with planned
incomplete data can be used to separate psychometric components of
developmental interest, including internal consistency reliability,
test-practice effects, factor stability, factor growth, and state
fluctuation. Practical analyses are proposed using a structural
equation model for longitudinal data on multiple groups with
different test-retest intervals. This approach is illustrated using
2 sets of data collected from students measured on the Woodcock-
Johnson-- Revised Memory and Reading scales. The results show how
alternative time-lag models can be fitted and interpreted with
univariate, bivariate, and multivariate data. Benefits,
limitations, and extensions of this structural time-lag approach
are discussed.
McArdle, J. J., FerrerCaja, E., Hamagami, F.,
&Woodcock, R. W. (2002). Comparative longitudinal structural
analyses of the growth and decline of multiple intellectual
abilities over the life span. Developmental Psychology,
38(1), 115-142. (click to
view)
Abstract: Latent growth curve techniques and
longitudinal data are used to examine predictions from the theory
of fluid and crystallized intelligence (Gf-Gc theory; J. L.
Horn & R. B. Cattell, 1966, 1967). The data examined are from a
sample (N = 1,200) measured on the Woodcock- Johnson
Psycho-Educational Battery—Revised (WJ–R). The
longitudinal structural equation models used are based on latent
growth models of age using two- occasion
“accelerated”data (e.g., J. J. McArdle & R. Q.
Bell, 2000; J. J. McArdle & R. W. Woodcock, 1997). Nonlinear
mixed-effects growth models based on a dual exponential rate yield
a reasonable fit to all life span cognitive data. These results
suggest that most broad cognitive functions fit a generalized curve
that rises and falls. Novel multilevel models directly comparing
growth curves show that broad fluid reasoning (Gf ) and
acculturated crystallized knowledge (Gc) have different
growth patterns. In all comparisons, any model of cognitive age
changes with only a single g factor yields an overly
simplistic view of growth and change over age.
Redden, S.
C., Forness, S. R., Ramey, S. L., Ramey, C. T., Brezausek, C. M.,
& Kavale, K. A. (2001). Children at- risk: Effects of a
four-year Head Start transition program on special education
identification. Journal of Child & Family Studies,
10(2), 255-270. (click to
view)
Abstract: Notes that while Head Start has decreased
special education placement, there has been little systematic data
until recently on identification of children in disability
categories following preschool. Two cohorts of 6,162 children (aged
3-5 yrs) were followed through 3rd grade. The measures used were
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised, Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-
Educational Battery-Revised, and Social Skills Rating System.
Approximately half of these children were provided transition
assistance from kindergarten through 3rd grade. This included
school transition and curricular modifications, parent involvement
activities, health screening or referrals, and family social
services. They were compared to a similar group of Head Start
children who did not receive such services beyond the Head Start
experience. Special education eligibility was determined from
school records in the spring of third grade. Only 0.89% of children
in the transition group were identified in the mental retardation
category compared to 1.26% in the non-transition group. In the
category of emotional disturbance, these same figures were 1.21%
and 1.65% respectively. Both differences were statistically
significant, but an opposite effect was found in the category of
speech or language impairment.
Speece, D.
L., C Ritchey, K. D., Cooper, D. H., Roth, F. P.,
&Schatschneiderd, C. (2004). Growth in early reading skills
from kindergarten to third grade. Contemporary
Educational Psychology , 29(3), 312-
332.(click to
view)
Abstract. We examined models of individual
change and correlates of change in the growth of reading skills in
a sample of 40 children from kindergarten through third grade. A
broad range of correlates was examined and included family
literacy, oral language, emergent reading, intelligence, spelling,
and demographic variables. Individual growth curve analysis was
used to model change in Letter Word Identi.cation (LWID), Word
Attack (WA), and Passage Comprehension (PC) subtests of the
Woodcock–Johnson Psychoeducational Battery – Revised.
Third grade LWID was predicted uniquely by family literacy,
phonological awareness, and emergent reading skills. Growth in LWID
was predicted uniquely by emergent reading skills. Phonological
awareness, spelling, and emergent reading were unique predictors of
third grade WA, whereas family literacy and emergent reading skills
uniquely predicted third grade PC. The general oral language factor
de.ned by semantic and syntactic variables did not contribute
signifcant unique variance in any of the models. Thus, the pattern
of results extends the model of emergent- to-
conventional literacy proposed by Whitehurst and Lonigan (1998) to
third grade and suggests that early contextual understandings
necessary for competent reading (family literacy and emergent
reading) become more in.uential as reading skills
develop.
Wood, F.
B. , Hill, D. F., Meyer, M. S., & Flowers, D. L. (2005).
Predictive assessment of reading. Annals of Dyslexia,
55(2), 193-216. (click to
view)
Abstract: Study 1 retrospectively analyzed
neuropsychological and psychoeducational tests given to N = 220
first graders, with follow-up assessments in third and eighth
grade. Four predictor constructs were derived: (1) Phonemic
Awareness, (2) Picture Vocabulary, (3) Rapid Naming, and (4) Single
Word Reading. Together, these accounted for 88%, 76%, 69%, and 69%
of the variance, respectively, in first, third, and eighth grade
Woodcock Johnson Broad Reading and eighth grade Gates-MacGinitie.
When Single Word Reading was excluded from the predictors, the
remaining predictors still accounted for 71%, 65%, 61%, and 65% of
variance in the respective outcomes. Secondary analyses of risk of
low outcome showed sensitivities/specificities of 93.0/91.0, and
86.4/84.9, respectively, for predicting which students would be in
the bottom 15% and 30% of actual first grade WJBR.
Sensitivities/specificities were 84.8/83.3 and 80.2/81.3,
respectively, for predicting the bottom 15% and 30% of actual third
grade WJBR outcomes; eighth grade outcomes had
sensitivities/specificities of 80.0/80.0 and 85.7/83.1,
respectively, for the bottom 15% and 30% of actual eighth grade
WJBR scores. Study 2 cross- validated the concurrent predictive
validities in an N = 500 geographically diverse sample of late
kindergartners through third graders, whose ethnic and racial
composition closely approximated the national early elementary
school population. New tests of the same four predictor domains
were used, together taking only 15 minutes to administer by
teachers; the new Woodcock-Johnson III Broad Reading standard score
was the concurrent criterion, whose testers were blind to the
predictor results. This cross-validation showed 86% of the variance
accounted for, using the same regression weights as used in Study
1. With these weights, sensitivity/specificity values for the 15%
and 30% thresholds were, respectively, 91.3/88.0 and 94.1/89.1.
These validities and accuracies are stronger than others reported
for similar intervals in the literature.
|
|