Distributed networks model
The alternate view is that timing is
distributed, meaning that many brain areas are capable of
temporal processing and that the area or areas involved depend on
the task and modality being used.
The above models [clock and spectral] represent
top-down approaches where timing is addressed by inferring a
computation and then implementing the computation with neurons. An
alternative bottom-up approach is to start with
biologically realistic assumptions and then to ask the
extent to which temporal processing can be found as an
emergent property. These models have no built-in
temporal processing or selectivity with ad hoc assumptions.
That is, they do not rely on explicitly setting oscillators,
synaptic or current-time constants, or some other variable that, in
effect, functions as a delay line.
In these network or state-dependent models,
timing does not arise from clocks or even from brain systems
specifically dedicated to temporal processing. Rather, the
evidence from the cerebellum, for example, illustrates howtiming
and performance on experimental tasks designed to study timing may
be mediated by computations that include temporal processing but
that are not accurately characterized as interval timers or
clocks.
In contrast, models based on network dynamics
may better generalize to the processing of more complex temporal
patterns. In state-dependent network models (see above;
Buonomano&Merzenich 1995, Buonomano 2000, Maass et al. 2002),
the current state of the network is always dependent on the
recent history of activity. Thus, in the above example, if the
third input arrives at 200 ms, the network will be in a different
state depending on whether the second pulse arrived at 50 or 150
ms. In these models, time- dependent properties, such as short-
term synaptic plasticity, slow PSPs (e.g., GABAB or NMDA-dependent
currents), or, potentially, slow conductance, function as state-
dependent memory traces of the recent stimulus history. In contrast
to single-cell models, these time-dependent properties are not
tuned for any particular interval; rather these states are
expressed as changes in the probability of different neurons
becoming activated.
Given the inherent temporal nature of our sensory
environment, and the continuous, real- time motor interaction with
our environment,we favor the view that temporal
and spatial information are generally processed together by the
same circuits, and that there is no centralized
clock for temporal processing on the scale of tens to
hundreds of ms. Additionally, we propose that temporal
processing does not rely on specialized mechanisms, such as
oscillators or arrays of elements, as with a spectrum of different
time constants. Rather, we believe that neural
circuits are inherently capable of processing temporal information
as a result of state-dependent changes in network
dynamics.
One such class of models,
state-dependent networks (SDNs), propose that
neural circuits are inherently capable of temporal processing as
a result of the natural complexity of cortical networks coupled
with the presence of time-dependent neuronal properties
(Buonomano and Merzenich, 1995; Buonomano, 2000; Maass et al.,
2002). This framework, based on well characterized cellular and
network properties, has been shown to be able to discriminate
simple temporal intervals on the millisecond scale, as well as
complex spatial-temporal patterns (Buonomano and Merzenich, 1995;
Buonomano, 2000; Maass et al., 2002).
Here we examine the mechanisms and nature of the
timing in this model and show that it encodes temporally
patterned stimuli as single ‘‘temporal
objects,’’ as opposed to the sum of the
individual component intervals. This generates the
counterintuitive prediction that we do not have access to the
objective (absolute) time of a given interval if it was immediately
preceded by another event.
In this model, there is no explicit or linear
measure of time like the tics of an oscillator or a continuously
ramping firing rate (see Discussion; Durstewitz, 2003).
Instead, time is implicitly encoded in the state of the
network— defined not only by which neurons are spiking, but
also by the properties that influence cell firing, such as the
membrane potential of each neuron and synaptic strengths at each
point in time. Thus, even in the absence of ongoing activity,
the recent stimulus history remains encoded in the network.
The standard model of temporal processing
postulates a single centralized internal clock, which relies on an
oscillator and an accumulator (counter) (Creelman, 1962; Treisman,
1963; Church, 1984; Grondin, 2001). The clock concept is
generally taken to imply that the passage of time is counted in
units that can be combined or compared linearly. In contrast,
SDN models propose that for spans on the scale of tens to
hundreds of milliseconds, time may be represented as specific
states of a neural network. Within this framework, a 50 ms
interval followed by a 100 ms interval is not encoded as the
combination of the two. Instead, the earlier stimulus interacts
with the processing of the 100 ms interval, resulting in the
encoding of a distinct temporal object. Thus, temporal
information is encoded in the context of the entire pattern, not as
conjunctions of the component intervals.
We propose here that cortical networks can
tell time as a result of time-dependent changes in synaptic and
cellular properties, which influence the population response to
sensory events in a history- dependent manner. This framework
is applicable to the processing of simple intervals as well as more
complex spatial-temporal patterns, and does not invoke any novel
hypothetical mechanisms at the neural and synaptic level.
Additionally, we propose that timing is not centralized, and can
potentially occur locally at both early and late stages of cortical
processing.