Attention has been widely reported to alter
estimates of time in the range of seconds (Hicks et al., 1976; Macar et al., 1994; Brown,
1997; Coull et al., 2004). Internal clock models
can account for attention- dependent effects in the second range by
assuming a gating mechanism that controls the number of events
generated by the oscillator that are counted by the accumulator
(Meck, 1984; Zakay and Tsal,
1989). In contrast, on the shorter time scale,
divided attention or cognitive load does not appear to specifically
alter temporal judgments (Rammsayer
and Lima, 1991; Lewis and Miall, 2003). Therefore,
the SDN model would be expected to be fairly insensitive to shifts
in attention.However, recent studies have revealed that temporal
distortions of short intervals can be produced by saccades or
stimulus features (Morrone et al.,
2005; Johnston et al., 2006). These studies
suggest that on short scales, timing is local, and are generally
consistent with the SDN model that predicts that temporal
processing could occur in a number of different cortical areas on
an as-needed basis.
An explanation often used for changes in
discriminative sensitivity is that of ``attention’’ ,
in the specific sense of exposing animals to situations that force
them to ``attend to’ ’ (or have their behaviour
brought under the control of) a particular stimulus dimension and
ignore irrelevant competing dimensions.
However, the word ``attention’’ has
another sense in everyday usage, which is perhaps more pertinent
here- -the idea of differential allocation of processing
resources in different conditions; thus when we pay particular
attention’’ , we allocate some sort of psychological
resource so that the processing of an event is particularly
precise, careful, and error-free (e.g. Kahneman, 1973). So, for
example, in the timing tasks discussed above, difficult
conditions cause subjects to process durations more carefully than
they do in ``easy’ ’ conditions, so time discrimination
becomes more sensitive.