Bold font designates new information
(02-01-08) since last revision.
Almeida, D. C. (2003). Predicting academic
aptitude for a high school learning- disabled sample.
Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences
& Engineering, 64(5-B), 2412.
Abstract: The current archival study examined the
ability of earlier Woodcock Johnson Revised achievement scores
and/or Wechsler Intelligence scores in predicting scholastic
Assessment Test (SAT) performance in the 12th grade for a learning
disabled sample. Subjects included 69 public high school classified
students from the suburban upper- middle class Westchester area.
Criteria for consideration of inclusion in the study were:
graduation from the special education program between 1998-2002,
WISC-III, WJ-R, WAIS-III and SAT test scores. The study was
designed to evaluate the predictive utility of various patterns of
performance already tenuously established for students with
learning difficulties. These patterns include ACID, SCAD and Verbal
< Performance splits. This study utilized a regression model to
predict performance on the SAT. It was determined whether or not a
student underachieved on the SAT. Performance on the WISC-III and
WAIS- III were evaluated to determine if patterns existed and to
determine, if such patterns predicted under- achievement on the
SAT. Changes in performance from the WISC-III to the WAIS-III that
predicted performance on SAT were also examined. The results of
this study provided a prediction model for school psychologists
working with learning disabled students, which may assist in
predicting which Learning Disabled students may not perform well on
the SAT. Such insight may generate more effective programming for
special education students. Implications for school psychologists
and suggestions for further research were also
discussed
Anjum, A.
(2005). The relationship between the Differential Ability
Scales and the Woodcock Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities
for children diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder. Dissertation Abstracts International,
65(9 ), 4859.
Abstract: Psychologists and other professionals often
assess students who exhibit symptoms of overactivity, impulsive
behavior and attention/concentration difficulties. Clinicians who
assess similar cognitive and perceptual processes of such
youngsters may sometimes get very conflicting results on the tests,
which purport to measure similar skills. These differences could be
a consequence of student variables, examiner- examinee variables,
examiner differences, differences in psychometric properties of the
tests, differences in task demands, or the difference in the
content being tapped by the two tests. The purpose of this study
was to investigate the validity of two intelligence measures the
Woodcock Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities (WJ III) and the
Differential Ability Scales (DAS) for use with children diagnosed
with ADHD, and to explore the amount of overlap or differences
between the similar broad cognitive abilities assessed by both
tests. These two tests were administered to twenty- seven
children diagnosed with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder.
The results from the present sample found the General Intellectual
Ability (GIA) of the WJ III, the General Conceptual Ability (GCA)
of the DAS, and the cluster scores from both the tests to be in the
Average range. After controlling for the Flynn Effect, no
significant differences were found between the respective global
Full Scale scores and the related cluster scores of the two tests.
The WJ III and the DAS associated moderately high, sharing 60%
variance. Moderate to moderately high correlations were found
between the related constructs, and moderate to moderately low
correlations were found between the related subtests of the two
tests were found. Implications of these findings and future avenues
for research investigating cognitive abilities in children
diagnosed with ADHD are discussed.
Ardoin, S. P. , Witt, J. C., Suldo, S. M.,
Connell, J. E., Koenig, J. L., Resetar, J. L. S. N. J., &
Williams, K. L. (2004). Examining the Incremental Benefits of
Administering a Maze and Three Versus One Curriculum-Based
Measurement Reading Probes When Conducting Universal Screening.
School Psychology Review, 33(2), 218-
233.
Abstract: A primary problem with special
education placement is that it is a solution that comes too late
for children who are first noticed by their kindergarten teachers
as lacking prerequisite skills. It is of critical importance that
these students, whose reading skills are low in comparison to their
peers, be accurately identified through universal screenings and be
provided with intervention. Most universal screening models suggest
the use of curriculum-based measurement (CBM). In the current
study, 77 third grade students were administered four CBM reading
probes, a maze, a group- administered standardized
achievement test, and reading subtests of the Woodcock-
Johnson-III. Analyses were conducted to evaluate whether it is
necessary to administer three rather than only one CBM reading
probe, the contribution of administering a maze in addition to CBM,
and the predictive validity of CBM versus a group- administered
standardized achievement test.
Bell, N. L., Rucker, M., Finch, A., &
Alexander, J. (2002). Concurrent validity of the Slosson Full-
Range Intelligence Test: Comparison with the Wechsler Intelligence
Scale for Children-- Third Edition and the Woodcock Johnson Tests
of Achievement-- Revised. Psychology in the Schools,
39(1), 31-38.
Abstract: This study examined the concurrent validity
of the Slosson Full-Range Intelligence Test (S- FRIT)
by comparing S- FRIT scores tot he scores of the
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children- Third Edition (WISC- III)
and the Woodcock- Johnson Tests of Achievement- Revised (WJ- R).
Data from 123 elementary students' screening and psychological
testing results were examined. Results revealed that the S-FRIT
scores were more related to overall intelligence, verbal, and math
abilities than nonverbal intelligence, reading, or written language
abilities. Further, it was found that 89% of the participants' S-
FRIT full- range IQ scores fell within one standard deviation of
their WISC- III FSIQ scores, with an average discrepancy of 7.6
points. Discrepancies between S- FRIT and WISC- III scores
were also examined by educational diagnostic categories and ability
levels. Limitations and suggestions for future research are
provided.
Botana, S.
M. (2001). A native (Spanish) language assessment of
intelligence and its role in the special education case study
evaluation of limited English proficient
students.Dissertation Abstracts International Section A:
Humanities & Social Sciences, 62((1-A)),
74.
Abstract: In the case study evaluation of limited
English proficient students, best practice recommendations call for
a bilingual evaluator. Empirical studies designed to examine the
results of these bilingual evaluations are also recommended. In
this dissertation research study 198 school- age Hispanic LEP
students were referred for a case study evaluation by their
classroom teachers. These students were administered a mixed
language, translated, adapted version of the WISC-III (unpublished
Kowsky version of the WISC-III), the Woodcock- Munoz Language
Survey in Spanish and English, and the Woodcock- Johnson and
Woodcock- Munoz Academic Achievement tests by a state certified
bilingual special education school psychologist who embodied the
"best practice" recommendations described in the literature.
Student performance was represented using a series of Pearson
correlations, the results of an exploratory factor analysis
performed on the data set, and the results of a multivariate
analysis of covariance in an effort to describe student profiles on
all measures. Significant group differences in performance across
all measures were documented suggesting that these bilingual
assessments are useful in distinguishing the disabled from the
nondisabled, and those with average ability from those with
significant mental impairments in a referred for special education
population. Descriptive statistics related to the unpublished
Kowsky version of the WISC- III, indicated significantly below
average Verbal IQ scores and low average range Performance IQ
scores for the Learning Disabled participants, Speech and Language
Impaired participants, and Not Eligible for Special Education
participants. The Educable Mentally Handicapped participants had a
flat profile with both Verbal IQ and Performance IQ scores in the
50- 65 point range. The mean point difference between the Verbal IQ
and the Performance IQ scales for the whole group was 12.3 (SD
12.15) and 17.2 (SD 13.05) for the Learning Disability group.
Subtest score analyses for all groups indicated that the Vocabulary
subtest received the lowest scaled score and the Coding subtest
received the highest scaled score. Exploratory factor analysis on
the Kowsky version of the WISC-III yielded a unique three- factor
structure. Empirically- based student profiles were described
and recommendations for interpreting data obtained during a
bilingual evaluation were offered.
Breslau,
N., Johnson, E. O., & Lucia, V. C. (2001). Academic achievement
of low birthweight children at age 11: The role of cognitive
abilities at school entry. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology, 29(4), 273- 279. (click to
view)
Carper, M. S. (2003). A concurrent validity
study comparing executive functioning of the Woodcock Johnson III
Tests of cognitive ability and the NEPSY. Dissertation
Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences & Engineering,
64(3-B), 1516.
Abstract: The purpose of this study was to examine the
concurrent validity between the Woodcock Johnson III Tests of
Cognitive Ability (WJ III COG; Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather,
2001) executive functioning tests and theNEPSY (Korkman, Kirk,
& Kemp, 1998) executive functioning tests within the same
population sample of nondisabled children. The tests of the WJ III
COG that are executive functioning tests are Concept Formation,
Planning, and Pair Cancellation. On the NEPSY the tests that make
up executive functioning are Tower and Design Fluency. A total of
60 participants (30 females and 30 males) were recruited to
participate in this study. Participants completed an IQ screener to
ensure intellectual functioning scores were 80 or above. Parents
provided background information, providing pertinent medical and
emotional information to rule out health and other behavioral
influences, as well as assuring that children were currently
passing all subjects in school. Participants additionally completed
the executive functioning tests comprising the WJ III COG and
NEPSY. Results indicated: no significant differences for mean
standard scores of the WJ III COG and NEPSY based on age or on sex;
although, there were significant correlations within tests of the
WJ III COG and NEPSY Design Fluency test; no significant
correlations found with any WJ III COG tests and NEPSY Tower test;
and WJ III COG and NEPSY correlations did not vary by sex or by
age. Implications of this study indicate that the WJ III COG and
NEPSY provide a time- and cost- effective means for comprehensive
assessment of executive functions and may enhance educational plan
development within the school setting. Careful study of the
referral information, as well as, the types of tasks that encompass
the executive functioning concept believed to be at deficit will
drive the test selection. This study has also provided research
data on the WJ III COG and NEPSY in the area of executive
functioning in which few studies were found to
exist.
Daleiden,
E. , Drabman, R. S., & Benton, J. (2002). The Guide to the
Assessment of Test Session Behavior: Validity in relation to
cognitive testing and parent-reported behavior problems in a
clinical sample. Journal of Clinical Child
&Adolescent Psychology, 31(2), 263- 271.(click to
view)
Abstract: Examined the validity of the Guide to
Assessment of Test Session Behavior (GATSB) in a clinical sample.
The purpose of this study was to extend validity research on the
GATSB to cognitive measures other than the Wechsler Intelligence
Scale for Children - IIIrd Edition (WISC-- III) and to
parent- reported child behavior problems. Test observations were
taken for 122 children (aged 6-16 yrs) who were referred to an
outpatient psychology clinic for psychoeducational evaluations.
GATSB scores demonstrated a moderate relation with general indexes
from the WISC- III, the Woodcock- Johnson Psychoeducational
Battery- - Revised, and the Wide Range Assessment of Memory and
Learning, but displayed low magnitude correlations with the Child
Behavior Checklist. These results expand existing data supporting
the validity of the GATSB and suggest that it may be adopted into a
clinic setting.
Duncan, J., & Rafter, E. M. (2005).
Concurrent and predictive validity of the Phelps Kindergarten
Readiness Scale- II.
Psychology in the
Schools,42(4), 355-259.(click to
view)
Abstract: The purpose of this research was to
establish the concurrent and predictive validity of the Phelps
Kindergarten Readiness Scale, Second Edition (PKRS- II; L. Phelps,
2003). Seventy- four kindergarten students of diverse ethnic
backgrounds enrolled in a northeastern suburban school participated
in the study. The concurrent administration of the PKRS-II and the
Woodcock- Johnson III Brief Intellectual Ability Scale
(R.W.Woodcock, K.S. McGrew, & N. Mather, 2001a) occurred in the
fall of the kindergarten year. To assess predictive validity,
theWoodcock Johnson III Tests of Achievement (R.W. Woodcock, K.S.
McGrew, & N. Mather, 2001b) was administered in the spring of
that year. All concurrent and predictive correlations were
significant. Based on the results of this study, the PKRS-II may be
used with confidence to screen for children who may be at risk for
academic difficulties.
Dumont, R. Willis, J. O., Farr, L. P.,
Mccarthy, T. & Price, L. (2000). The Relationship
between the Differential Ability Scales (DAS) and the Woodcock-
Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability- Revised (WJ-R COG) for Students
Referred for Special Education Evaluations. Journal
Of Psychoeducational Assessment,18(1), 27-
38.(click to
view)
Abstract. The Differential Ability Scales (DAS)
and the Woodcock- Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability- Revised (WJ-
R COG standard battery) were administered to 81
children referred for special education services evaluation. Both
the General Conceptual Ability (GCA) and the Nonverbal Reasoning
scores of the DAS were significantly lower than the WJ- R COG Broad
Cognitive Ability- Standard score (BCA-STD). The DAS Verbal and
Spatial Composite scores were not significantly different from the
BCA-STD. Low to moderate correlations were found between related
constructs on the two tests.
Elliott,
J., Lee, S. W., & Tollefson, N. (2001). A reliability and
validity study of the dynamic indicators of basic early literacy
skills- modified. School Psychology Review,
30(1), 33- 49. (click to
view)
Abstract. This study examined the psychometric
properties of a set of preliteracy measures modified from the
Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills (DIBELS) with a
sample of 75 kindergarten students. The modified battery (called
DIBELS-M) includes measures of Letter Naming Fluency, Sound Naming
Fluency, Initial Phoneme Ability, and Phonemic Segmentation
Ability. These measures were assessed through repeated
administrations in 2-week intervals at the end of the kindergarten
year. Interrater reliability estimates and coefficients of
stability and equivalence for three of the measures ranged from .80
to the mid .90s with about one- half of the coefficients above .90.
Correlations between DIBELS-M scores and criterion measures of
phonological awareness, standardized achievement measures, and
teacher ratings of achievement yielded concurrent validity
coefficients ranging from .60 to .70. Hierarchical regression
analysis showed that the four DIBELS- M measures accounted for 73%
of the variance in scores on the Skills Cluster of the Woodcock-
Johnson Psychoeducational Battery-- Revised (WJ- R). The results of
the analysis support the use of the DIBELS-M measures for
identification of kindergarten students who are at- risk for
reading failure and for progress monitoring. The contributions of
the study, including psychometric analysis of the DIBELS-M with a
new sample and formation of composite scores, are discussed in
relation to the extant literature.
Erford, B. T., & Dutton, J. L. (2005).
Technical Analysis of the Slosson Phonics and Structural Analysis
Test.Educational and Psychological Measuremen2005,
65(6), 1011- 1025. (click to
view)
Abstract. Technical characteristics of Slosson
Phonics and Structural Analysis Test (SP- SAT) scores were studied
using three independent samples of boys and girls aged 6 years, 6
months, through 9 years, 11 months. A decision efficiency study
(n = 100) resulted in a total predictive value of .86
(sensitivity = .95, specificity = .77, positive predictive power =
.80, negative predictive power = .94). Item difficultywas .65
(median), and item discrimination was .52 (median). Interitem
consistency was r = .93, .93, and .96 for scores on the
Phonics Index, Structural Analysis Index, and SP-SAT total standard
score, respectively (n = 375), whereas 2- week test-retest
reliability was r = .90, .88, and .90 for scores on the same
scales, respectively (n = 165). The SP-SAT yielded moderate
to high concurrent validity coefficients when compared with the
Wide- Range Achievement Test–Third Edition Reading subtest;
Woodcock-Johnson: Tests of Achievement–Third Edition Letter-
Word Identification,Word Attack, Passage Comprehension, and Writing
Samples subtests; and Woodcock- Johnson: Tests of
Achievement–Revised Dictation subtest. Practice and research
implications are discussed.
Feldmann, G. M., Kelly, R. M., & Diehl, V.
A. (2004). An interpretative analysis of five commonly used
processing speed measures. Journal of Psychoeducational
Assessment, 22(2), 151-
163.(click
to view)
Abstract. Processing speed subtests are
components of Revised (Woodcock & Johnson, 1989), and widely
used intellectual assessment insuu- Speed of Information Processing
from the men-. Many researchers interpret these me% Differential
Ability Scales (Elliott, 1990) were ures as assessing a unitary
construct, hut there administered to 102 volunteer participants. is
a question concerning the constructs. Using regression analyses,
performance on assessed by these measures and, ultimately, each of
these tests was predicted by motor their interpretative utility.
Coding and Symbol speed and/or number facility factors. Search
from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale Individual differences in
motor speed were for Children- Third Edition
(Wechsler, 1991), found to he related to each of the five process
Visual Matching and Cross Out from the ing speed measures, whereas
number facility Woodcock Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability- was
related to three of the measures.
Foley, D.
R. (2004). Relationships among cognitive abilities,
vocational aptitudes and vocational interests in at- risk eighth
graders. Dissertation Abstracts International Section A:
Humanities & Social Sciences, 65(1-A), 67.
Abstract: Relative to instrument choice and target
population, this study was the first empirical attempt to explore
the relationships among Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) cognitive
abilities, as measured by Woodcock- Johnson, Third
Edition (WJ- III COG) and vocational aptitudes and interests, as
measured by the Occupational Aptitude Survey and Interest
Schedule-2 (OASIS-2 AS) and the Career Decision Making System- R,
Level One (CDM-R), respectively, of at-risk adolescents prior to
their entrance into high school. The sample included 169 eighth
graders. Roughly 66% were male and 47% were members of ethnic
minority groups. Pearson correlations revealed many significant
pair- wise relationships among the three constructs; however, most
were small (r le; .29) and of limited usefulness. Consistent with
the literature, moderate to strong relationships (.31 le; r le;
.53) were found between General Intellectual Ability (g) and
Comprehension- Knowledge (Gc) and General and Verbal Aptitudes,
between Visual Processing (Gv) and Spatial Aptitude, and between
Processing Speed (Gs) and Perceptual Aptitude. Previously
unreported relationships (.29 le; 5 r le; .50) were found between
(g) and Spatial and Perceptual Aptitudes, between Fluid Reasoning
(Gf) and General, Verbal and Spatial Aptitudes, and between Long-
Term Retrieval (Glr) and Verbal Aptitude. Multiple regression
analyses revealed that cognitive abilities together accounted for
the greatest variance in General and Verbal Aptitude scores;
however, only 24% and 28%, respectively. Cognitive abilities and
aptitudes together accounted for only small percentages of the
variances in Crafts (10%), Scientific (7%) and Social (14%)
interest scores. Sex was a significant predictor of Crafts and
Social interests. No single cognitive or aptitude factor was found
to be a significant predictor of Scientific interests. Consistent
with the literature, the results of this study indicated that
vocational aptitudes scores cannot be adequately predicted from
intelligence test scores, and that vocational interests are largely
unrelated to cognitive abilities and vocational aptitudes. In order
to measures these three constructs reliably and validly, they must
be measured directly. Although characteristics of the sample and
the aptitude and interest tests limit their generalizability, the
results of this study supported the need for similar studies using
CHC theory as a foundation.
Floyd, R. G., Bergeron, R., McCormack, A. C.,
Anderson, J. L., & HargroveOwens, G. L. (2005). Are Cattell-
Horn-Carroll broad ability composite scores exchangeable across
batteries?School Psychology Review, 34(3),
329-357. (click to
view)
Abstract. Many school psychologists use the
Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) theory of cognitive abilities to guide
their interpretation of scores from intelligence test batteries.
Some may frequently assume that composite scores purported to
measure the same CHC broad abilities should be relatively similar
for individuals no matter what subtests or batteries were
administered to obtain these scores. This study examined this
assumption using six samples of preschool children, school- age
children, or adults who completed two or more intelligence test
batteries. From these samples, composites measuring the broad
abilities Crystallized Intelligence, Visual Processing, Fluid
Reasoning, and Processing Speed were compared to examine their
exchangeability. Results indicate that most CHC broad ability
composites produced scores that were not as exchangeable for
individuals as may have been assumed by some. Discussion focuses on
the influence of score reliability and on the interaction between
examinee characteristics and the tasks used to measure the broad
abilities.
Floyd, R., McCormack, A., Ingram, E, Davis, A
&Bergeron, R (2006). Relations Between the Woodcock-
Johnson III Clinical Clusters and Measures of Executive Functions
From the Delis- Kaplan Executive Function System. Journal of
Psychoeducational Assessment, 24 (4), 303- 317(click to
view)
Abstract. This study examined the convergent
relations between scores from four clinical clusters from the
Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities (WJ III) and
measures of executive functions using a sample of school-aged
children and a sample of adults. The WJ III clinical clusters
included the Working Memory, Cognitive Fluency, Broad Attention,
and Executive Processes clusters, and the measures of executive
functions were from the Delis- Kaplan Executive Function
System (D-KEFS). Across both samples, all clinical clusters
demonstrated evidence of statistically significant and moderate
positive relations with at least some measures of executive
functions. The Executive Processes cluster demonstrated relations
with measures of executive functions that tended to be the
strongest and most consistent of the WJ III clinical clusters. When
these relations between the clinical clusters and the measures of
executive functions were contrasted with the relations between the
WJ III Comprehension- Knowledge cluster and the same measures of
executive functions, results called into question the distinction
between process and content but suggested that there is a common
ability or common abilities underlying performance across all of
the WJ III and D-KEFS measures.
Ganga, V. (2005). Adolescents with low
academic intrinsic motivation: Relation to educational
performance.Dissertation Abstracts International,
43(1), 328.
Abstract: The construct of low academic intrinsic
motivation was examined in a longitudinal investigation. Children
with low academic intrinsic motivation were compared with their
cohort peers in the subject areas of reading and math. Students
with the lowest motivation in reading and math were compared on two
outcome measures, The Woodcock Johnson Psychoeducational Battery
and Scholastic Aptitude Test scores to test the generality.
Repeated measures ANOVA and t- tests revealed that students with
low academic intrinsic motivation scored significantly lower on the
outcome measures than their cohort peer
comparison.
Hanzel, E.
P. (2003). Assessment of cognitive abilities in high-functioning
children with autistic disorder: A comparison of the WISC-III and
Leiter-R. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section
B: The Sciences & Engineering, 64(3-B),
1492.
Abstract: This study extends present research on the
assessment of intellectual functioning and patterns in cognitive
abilities in individuals with high-functioning autistic disorder
(AD- HF). This is the first study to examine the validity of the
recent revision of the Leiter International Performance Scale
(Leiter-R) when used to assess individuals with autistic disorder.
This study further explored the patterns of strengths and
weaknesses of cognitive functioning in children with autism based
on the nonverbal ability factors of fluid reasoning and broad
visualization from the Horn- Cattell theory of intelligence. Twenty
children with AD- HF matched on age and gender to twenty
normal control children were the participants. The dependent
variables consisted of each child's scores on the following
measures: the Visualization and Reasoning battery of the Leiter- R,
the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Third Edition
(WISC-III), the Nonverbal Reasoning factor of the Differential
Abilities Scale (DAS), and the Visual Processing factor of the
Woodcock-Johnson - Revised Cognitive Battery (WJ-R). A supplemental
analysis of the relationship of adaptive functioning to
intellectual abilities was also performed using the Scale of
Independent Behavior - Revised (SIB- R) and the Vineland Adaptive
Behavior Scale. There was mixed evidence of the validity of the
Leiter- R in the assessment of children with autistic disorder.
Comparisons of the Leiter-R Full Scale, Fluid Reasoning factor and
Visualization factor scores all showed high correlations to their
corresponding ability measures on the WISC- III, DAS, and WJ- R,
indicating that he factor scores on the Leiter-R do indeed measure
fluid reasoning (Gf) and visualization (Gv). However, since there
were also strong correlations of the Gf and Gv factors to each
other, the validity evidence indicates a broader construct may
being measured that encompasses both fluid reasoning (Gf) and
visualization (Gv). The Fluid Reasoning score of the Leiter-R
correlated slightly higher to the Full Scale and Verbal scores of
the WISC-III than the Leiter-R Full Scale score, indicating it may
be an even more reliable measure of general intellectual
functioning than the Leiter- R Full Scale score. The results
of this study also show an overall balance in the fluid reasoning
and visualization abilities in children with AD- HF. Though higher
visualization scores than fluid reasoning scores were predicted for
the children with AD- HF, this study indicates that they do not
have isolated visual- spatial skills, stimuli.
The results also indicated that the adaptive functioning of
children with AD-HF is more consistent with their verbal skills
than their nonverbal abilities. The overall findings of this study
indicate that the Leiter-R is a valid measure of general
intellectual functioning, fluid reasoning and visualization in
children with AD-HF. This study further shows that, though children
with AD-HF may have strong visual- spatial abilities, they can also
possess similar
Hargrave,
J. L. (2005). The relationship between executive functions and
broad written language skills in students ages 12 to 14 years
old. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The
Sciences & Engineering, 65(8-B), 4320. (click to
view)
Abstract: The purpose of this investigation was to
explore the relationship between executive functions and written
language skills. Five hundred and forty- three students between the
ages 12 and 14 were administered the Planning, Retrieval Fluency,
Pair Cancellation, and Number Reversal subtests from the Woodcock-
Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities - Third Edition (WJIII;
Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001a) and the subtests included
under the Broad Written Language cluster of the Woodcock-Johnson
Tests of Achievement - Third Edition (WJIII; Woodcock, McGrew,
& Mather, 2001b). The sample was taken from the larger sample
used to norm the WJIII tests. Relationships were explored between
the executive function subtest scores and Broad Written Language
scores and were found to be significantly correlated. The four
executive function measures and gender were entered simultaneously
into a multiple regression equation and found to significantly
predict Broad Written Language scores. Further examination revealed
that all the composites with the exception of the Planning subtest
significantly predicted Broad Written Language skills. Subtests
measuring the executive functions working memory and attention
(Number Reversal and Pair Cancellation) were the most significant
contributors when the composite scores were examined. Results
provide researchers with a foundation to further investigate the
underlying executive functions that may help or hinder students'
ability to produce quality written products and eventually design
intervention studies based on areas of executive functioning.
Limitations of the study are presented as well as implications for
research and practice and directions for future
research.
Hawkins,
B. A., Eklund, S. J., James, D. R., & Foose, A. K. (2003).
Adaptive behavior and cognitive function of adults with Down
syndrome: Modeling change with age. Mental Retardation,
41(1), 7-28 (click to
view)
Abstract. Fifty-eight adults with Down syndrome
(ages 31 to 56 years at time of first testing, mean age, 43.5) were
assessed longitudinally over 10 years for the purpose of modeling
aging- related change in cognitive function and adaptive behavior.
Cognitive function was assessed seven times using the Woodcock-
Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability- Revised Early Development
Battery. Adaptive behavior was evaluated five times using the
Inventory for Client and Agency Planning. Multi- level
(hierarchical linear) modeling procedures were used to model change
with age. Findings provided further evidence of changes in
performance with age and included selected effects for participants
who completed the 10 years of study and those who were lost to
follow-up as well as for age cohorts.
Hills, K.
J. (2005). An evaluation of universal screening measures in
reading. Dissertation Abstracts International,
66(4), 2336.
Abstract: New federal initiatives and reports are
calling attention to the importance of conducting universal
screenings in the initial years of students' schooling and
providing early intervention to students whose performance is
discrepant from their peers. The current identification system of
teacher referral and special education evaluation using an Intra-
Individual Achievement Discrepancy (IAD) model is not an effective
means of early identification. Because a significant discrepancy is
needed between student's ability (IQ) and achievement scores, the
IAD model rarely is able to identify students early in their
academic career when intervention would be most effective so
instead students must “wait to fail.”
Identifying efficient and valid universal screening measures is
thus a critical issue in education. The primary purpose of the
current study was to evaluate the validity of RR and R-
CBM, two different universal screening measures, by
examining each measures' relationship to students overall reading
achievement as measured by the Woodcock Johnson- III reading
subtests (Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001) after
accounting for intelligence, age, and grade. In addition, the
current study evaluated the differences between using challenging
versus similar level R- CBM probes for universal screening.
Finally, the present study extends previous research in the area of
teacher ratings by comparing the precision of teacher judgments of
accuracy versus fluency on both R-CBM and RR measures. Overall
results indicated that both R- CBM and RR significantly related to
students' overall reading achievement as indicated by the WJ- III
While both R- CBM and RR accounted for a significant amount of
variance in students' reading achievement, R-CBM was a stronger
predictor of reading achievement. Comparisons of similar versus
challenging R-CBM passages indicated no significant differences
between passages' relationship with reading achievement. Finally,
analyses of teacher ratings indicated that teachers may be able to
recognize relative differences in level of performance across
studen’ts, however, they tend to be less accurate at
predicting specific levels of student oral reading
fluency.
Huff, E., Sorenson, J., & Dancer, J.
(2002). Relation of reading rate and rapid automatic naming among
third graders.Perceptual Motor Skills, 95(3 Pt
1), 925-926.(click to
view)
Abstract. The relation of reading rate and rapid
automatic naming (RAN) for pictures was investigated for 31 third
graders. Reading rate was measured as the number of seconds
required for reading aloud a third- grade passage.
Rapid automatic naming was assessed with the RAN portion of the
Woodcock Johnson I11 Tests of Cognitive Abilities. A moderate
correlation of -.61 between the sets of scores suggests RAN
assessment may be useful in screening for reading deficits in
third- grade children.
Jones, W., Loe, S., Krach, K., Rager, R.
& Jones, H. (2008). Automated Neuropsychological
Assessment Metrics (ANAM) and Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of
Cognitive Ability: A concurrent valildity
study. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
22, 305–320. (click
to view)
This study examines the relationship between a
computerized neuropsychological assessment battery, the Automated
Neuropsychological Assessment Metrics (ANAM) and a widely used
ability measure, Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Ability
(WJ-III). Results indicated substantial relationship between the
ANAM throughput (accuracy /response time) scores and the WJ-III
Cognitive Efficiency cluster. An unexpectedly strong relationship
was evident between accuracy scores on the ANAM Logical Reasoning
scale and the WJ-III General Intellectual Ability score, purporting
to measure the g factor. The findings support the viability of the
ANAM as a time- and cost-effective tool for appraisal of cognitive
function.
Joseph, L. M., McCachran, M. E., &
Naglieri, J. A. (2003). PASS cognitive processes, phonological
processes, and basic reading perrformance for a sample of referred
primary- grade children. Journal of Research in Reading,
26(3), 304- 314. (click to
view)
Abstract. The purpose of this study was to
explore the relationships among cognitive processing, phonological
processing and basic reading skill performance. Cognitive theorists
propose that Planning, Attention, Simultaneous and Successive
(PASS) processes are related to various phonological skills. A
sample of 62 Primary Grade children referred for reading problems
were administered measures of cognitive processes (Cognitive
Assessment System), phonological processes (Comprehensive Test of
Phonological Processing) and basic reading achievement (Woodcock-
Johnson Tests of Academic Achievement- III). Findings
indicated that some cognitive processes were significantly related
to phonological processes as well as basic reading skills. The
strongest relationships were found between phonological memory and
successive processes and between phonological awareness and basic
reading performance.
Kenney, K.
M. (2002). Parental acculturation and child language preference as
predictors of cognitive ability and academic achievement in
Asian-American children. Dissertation Abstracts
International: Section B: The Sciences & Engineering,
62((8- B)), 3843.
Abstract: The purpose of this study was to investigate
relationships among parental acculturation and language preference
in Asian-American children and scores on tests of cognitive ability
and academic achievement. This study used the Suinn- Lew Asian
Self- Identity Scale (SL-ASIA) to quantify five possible levels of
acculturation in the child's parents. The Short Acculturation Scale
for Hispanic Youth (SASH-Y) was adapted for Asian- Americans and
was used to assess the child's language preference. Cognitive
ability was measured using the Stanford Binet: Fourth Edition
(SB:FE). Academic achievement was measured by the Woodcock-Johnson
Revised. Participants were one- hundred-forty-eight Asian- American
students (ages 5 to 6 years) who completed the cognitive,
achievement, and language preference tests. One parent of each
child completed the SL- ASIA. The children were administered
the adapted version of the Short Acculturation Scale for Hispanic
Youth (SASH-Y) for Asians. Results indicated that parental
acculturation had a positive correlation with reported language
preference in that parents who were highly "Western Identified" had
children who expressed preferences for the use of English in
multiple contexts. Parental acculturation also had positive
correlations with the Broad Knowledge Cluster of the Woodcock
Johnson- Revised. A partial correlation was conducted to show a
nonsignificant correlation between parental acculturation and
scores on the Stanford Binet: Fourth Edition and Woodcock Johnson
Tests of Achievement when child language preference is held
constant. Results only confirmed these hypothesis on scores that
loaded for crystallized intelligence (Gc) such as the Verbal
Reasoning Factor of the Stanford Binet: Fourth Edition, the Broad
Knowledge Cluster of the Woodcock Johnson- Revised, as well as the
mean of the Broad Knowledge and Skills Clusters. The implications
of evaluating language preference/proficiency in psychoeducational
assessments are discussed. The use of the English language in
multiple contexts among culturally diverse children can be a
powerful indicator of overall functioning. Greater awareness of
language preference can be important for clinicians evaluating
culturally diverse children and for parents who often dictate the
level of acculturation for their children. The importance of
evaluating language as the primary medium of acculturation is
discussed. Behavioral definitions of acculturation as opposed to
those that are more value- oriented are
delineated.
Lee, E.,
Murry, V. M., Brody, G., & Parker, V. (2002). Maternal
resources, parenting, and dietary patterns among rural African
American children in single-parent families.Public Health
Nursing, 19(2), 104- 111.(click to
view)
Abstract: Tested 2 hypotheses: (1) that maternal
education, perceived family resources, and the importance of family
routines would be related to children's dietary patterns and (2)
that dietary patterns would be associated with children's cognitive
and physical abilities. 159 African American single-mother families
with a child (6-9 yrs) living in rural areas, most of whom lived in
poverty. Children's eating patterns were assessed using a
selfreport questionnaire administered to the mother in an
interview. Children's cognitive ability was measured by the
Woodcock Johnson Psycho- Educational Instrument and the Harter
Perceived Competence Scale for Children. For male children, the
mother's higher education was related to more adequate eating
patterns at home, and more perceived family resources were related
to the likelihood of taking vitamin supplements. For female
children, greater milk intake was positively related to cognitive
outcomes, including applied problem, passage comprehension,
calculation, synonym identification, antonym identification, and
quantitative concept scores. Milk intake was related to more
optimal cognitive development. The results of this study support
the literature related to the importance of nutrition for cognitive
and physical abilities in children.
Leverett,
P. J., Lassiter, K. S., & Buchanan, G. M. (2002). Correlations
for the Stroop Color and Word Test with measures of reading and
language achievement. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
94, 459-466.
Lewandowski, L. J., Codding, R. S., Kleinmann,
A. E., &Tucker, K. L. (2003). Assessment of reading rate in
postsecondary students. Journal of Psychoeducational
Assessment, 21(2), 134- 144. (click to
view)
Abstract. There has been a steady increase in
students with disabilities attending college and making requests
for test accommodations. Most requests are for extended time,
presumably due to slow reading speed. Tests of reading rate for
adults have been criticized for poor psychometric adequacy, and no
current norms exist regarding the expected reading rate for college
students. This study examined reading rate via two methods, as well
as their relationship to other reading measures. Ninety typical
college students (67% female) were administered the Nelson Denny
Reading Test (Reading Rate [NDRR] and Comprehension [NDC] tests),
the Woodcock Johnson-III Reading Fluency (RF) and Word Attack (WA)
tests, and three oral reading probes used to obtain words read
correctly per minute (WRCM). On average, college students read 189
words correctly per minute based on oral reading probes and 231 wpm
based on NDRR. The WRCM measure was more strongly related to RF and
WA than was NDRR and was a better predictor of reading
comprehension (NDC). It would appear that WRCM may be a better
measure of reading rate than the often used, much criticized,
Nelson Denny Reading Rate. Additional research on WRCM with adults
is recommended.
Lohman,
D. (2003, March). The Woodcock-Johnson III and the Cognitive
Abilities Test (Form 6): A Concurrent Validity Study
(click to
view)
Abstract: This study investigated the
concurrent validity of the Woodcock-Johnson III (WJ-III; Woodcock,
McGrew, & Mather, 2001) and Form 6 of the Cognitive Abilities
Test (CogAT; Lohman & Hagen, 2001). A total of 178 students in
grades 2, 5, and 9 were administered 13 tests from the WJ-III and
the appropriate level of the CogAT. Interbattery confirmatory
factor analyses showed that the general factors on the two
batteries correlated r = .82. Correlations between broad-group
clusters on the WJ-III and battery-level scores on the CogAT
generally supported the construct interpretations of each, but also
suggested important differences in the abilities measured by both
batteries.
Mayes, S.
D. , Calhoun, S. L., & Lane, S. E. (2005). Diagnosing
children's writing disabilities: Different tests give different
results. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 101(1),
72-78
McCabe, P.
P., Margolis, H., & Barenbaum, E. (2001). A comparison of
Woodcock- Johnson Psycho- Educational Battery- Revised and
Qualitative Reading Inventory-II instructional reading levels.
Reading & Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning
Difficulties, 17(4), 279- 289.(click to
view)
Abstract: To examine whether or not norm-referenced
reading tests (NRT) or informal reading inventories (IRI) yielded
comparable, interchangable results, this study hypothesized that
4th grade male of below average reading ability would obtain
functionally discrepant reading scores on the reading portion of
the Woodcock- Johnson Psycho- Educational Battery-Revised (WJ-R),
an NRT, and the Qualitative Reading Inventory-II (QRI- II), an IRI.
The QRI-II and the reading subtests of the WJ-R were administered
to 34 4th- grade males (mean age 10.2 yrs) reading at or below the
25th percentile on the Iowa Test of Basic Skills. Spearman rank-
order correlation coefficients indicate that WJ-R reading scores
and QRI-II oral instructional reading levels were moderately and
significantly related. 50% of children obtained identical
instructional levels on the WJ-R and QRI- II, while 50%
demonstrated differences of half a year or more. For the portion of
students who obtained different WJ-R and QRI-II instructional
levels, WJ-R levels exceeded QRI-II levels 92.1% of the time.
Implications for placing poor readers in instructional level
reading materials are discussed.
McCabe, P.
P., Margolis, H., & Barenbaum, E. (2001). A comparison of
Woodcock- Johnson Psycho- Educational Battery- Revised and
Qualitative Reading Inventory-II instructional reading levels.
Reading & Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning
Difficulties, 17(4), 279-289.
Abstract: To examine whether or not norm-referenced
reading tests (NRT) or informal reading inventories (IRI) yielded
comparable, interchangable results, this study hypothesized that
4th grade male of below average reading ability would obtain
functionally discrepant reading scores on the reading portion of
the Woodcock- Johnson Psycho- Educational Battery-Revised (WJ-R),
an NRT, and the Qualitative Reading Inventory-II (QRI- II), an IRI.
The QRI-II and the reading subtests of the WJ-R were administered
to 34 4th- grade males (mean age 10.2 yrs) reading at or below the
25th percentile on the Iowa Test of Basic Skills. Spearman rank-
order correlation coefficients indicate that WJ-R reading scores
and QRI-II oral instructional reading levels were moderately and
significantly related. 50% of children obtained identical
instructional levels on the WJ-R and QRI- II, while 50%
demonstrated differences of half a year or more. For the portion of
students who obtained different WJ-R and QRI-II instructional
levels, WJ-R levels exceeded QRI-II levels 92.1% of the time.
Implications for placing poor readers in instructional level
reading materials are discussed.
Mockler,
J. L. (2004). Predicting reading achievement in children: The
significance of naming speed, phonological awareness, cognitive
ability, processing speed, and neuroanatomy.Dissertation
Abstracts International Section A: Humanities & Social
Sciences, 64( 9-A), 3191.
Abstract: This study investigated the relationship
among behavioral and neuroanatomical predictors of reading
achievement in a group of school-age children. Specifically,
relationships between naming speed and other predictors of reading
(i.e., phonological awareness, cognitive ability, and processing
speed) were investigated to determine how these variables influence
naming speed's prediction of reading achievement. In addition, this
study examined specific neuroanatomical regions and their
relationship to these predictors. A sample of 57 children, between
the ages of 7 and 11 years, were administered three measures of
naming speed (i.e., RAN Colors, Numbers, and Letters tasks), two
measures of phonological awareness (i.e., the Elision and the
Lindamood Auditory Conceptualization tests [LAC]), a measure of
cognitive ability (Woodcock Johnson- Test of Cognitive Ability-
Revised), a battery of elementary cognitive tasks (Visual
Inspection Time [IT], and Simple, Choice, and Odd- Man-Out Reaction
Time [RT]), and three subtests from the Woodcock- Johnson Tests of
Achievement- Revised to assess reading skills and performance. In
addition, all participants received a magnetic resonance image
(MRI) of their brain to measure specific neuroanatomical regions,
including surface area of the pars triangularis, planum temporale
and parietale, Heschl's gyrus, and the corpus callosum. Volumetric
measurements were also made of the anterior lobe of the cerebellum
and the cerebrum. There were three major findings in this study.
First, consistent with previous research, the results of this study
indicate significant contributions of both naming speed and
phonological awareness to the prediction of reading achievement.
However, in this study, phonological awareness emerged as the
strongest predictor overall, with a limited contribution from
naming speed, for all of the specific reading skills assessed.
Second, the results suggest that cognitive ability is an important
predictor of reading achievement and may share some overlapping
variance with naming speed. In addition, the relationship between
naming speed and reading appears unrelated to processing speed.
Finally, the neuroanatomical findings in this study suggest
significant relationships among regions in the brain and predictors
of reading achievement. However, it is more likely that the
variability in reading skill can be better accounted for by a
combination of behavioral and biological factors. Implications for
future research are discussed
Norman-Prater, K. (2004). A WISC-III short form and the
Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities: Correlations
with gifted children. Dissertation Abstracts
International: Section B: The Sciences & Engineering,
65(6- B), 3217.
Abstract: The overall purpose of this study was to
investigate the nature of the relationship between a recently
revised, multidimensional intelligence test (WJ III COG) and a
short form of an older, well- established intelligence test
(WISC-III) with intellectually gifted children. As such, this study
examined the implications of using a theoretically and empirically
sound choice (WJ III COG) as compared to a more practical
alternative (i.e., WISC-III short form); it also explored the
impact of different cut- off and eligibility criteria upon
eligibility decisions. Participants were solicited from a group of
75 students who had been nominated for a gifted program at a small
elementary school located on the urban fringe of a mid- size city
in the Midwest. Thirty- five students, ranging in age from 9 years,
2 months to 11 years, 1 month, participated in this study. The
sample included 15 students who were admitted into the program and
20 students who were deemed ineligible. The WISC-III short form
exhibited a positive relationship with the WJ III COG, as its FSIQ
estimate correlated significantly with both the WJ III COG GIA-Std
and BIA scores, accounting for approximately 33% and 35% of the
variance, respectively. The eligible group performed significantly
higher on the WISC- III short form than the WJ III COG, whereas the
ineligible group performed consistently across all global measures
of intelligence. The eligibility of 46% of the sample varied as a
result of the test and restrictiveness of the cut-off criteria.
More students were identified as intellectually gifted when
flexible, rather than strict, cut-off criteria were used to make
eligibility decisions. Moreover, the eligibility of approximately
63% of the participants varied as a result of the test and whether
eligibility criteria involved general and specific intellectual
abilities or solely general intellectual ability. More students
were deemed eligible when general and specific intellectual
abilities were considered as compared to decisions based only on
general intellectual ability
Osmon, D.,
& Jackson, R. (2002). Inspection time and IQ: Fluid or
perceptual aspects of intelligence. Intelligence ,
30(2), 119- 128. (click to
view)
Abstract: Past research has found an association
between inspection time (IT) and fluid intelligence using measures
confounded with visual processing (e.g., Wechsler PIQ or Ravens
Progressive Matrices). The present study related IT to intelligence
using a measure (Woodcock- Johnson-Revised, WJ-R) that has
nonconfounded factors of mental ability in order to determine
whether the association is based upon fluid IQ or perceptual
processes. 37 undergraduate students were given fluid,
crystallized, and visual processes subtests from the Woodcock-
Johnson and a visual IT task. Stepwise multiple regression and
partial correlations revealed that IT was related only to fluid
intelligence, supporting the notion that IT reflects some
fundamental underlying aspect of intelligence such as neural
processing efficiency.
Pedersen,
L. L. (2005). The relationship between behavioral and
performance-based measures of executive function in preschool
children. Dissertation Abstracts International,
66(3), 1748.
Abstract: his study explored the relationship between
parent and teacher ratings of the Behavior Rating Inventory of
Executive Function - Preschool Version (BRIEF-P) and performance
based measures of executive function in preschool children. A
sample of 93 preschool children [age range 3-4); 58% male, 42%
female) was administered Concept Formation and Numbers Reversed
from the Woodcock Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities - Third
Edition and Visual Attention, Statue, Sentence Repetition and
Verbal Fluency subtests from the NEPSY. Additionally, teacher and
parent ratings on the BRIEF- P were obtained for each child
participant. Regarding test performance and ratings scales based on
age, Hotelling's tests were conducted and no significant
differences were found. No significant differences for parent
ratings based on gender were found. However, teachers were found to
rate males higher than females on all Index scales and the
Composite. Significant sex related differences were found in test
performance of Visual Attention subtest, with females performing
better than males. A Hotelling's test was conducted to
examine overall mean correlations between parent and teacher
ratings. Results indicate a significant difference between parent
and teacher ratings with parents reporting higher levels of problem
behaviors than teachers on all three Index scales and the Composite
scale. Higher than expected correlation coefficients were found
between parent and teacher ratings with coefficients ranging from
.322 to .427. Correlation coefficients comparing parent ratings and
teacher Index and Composite ratings to test performance resulted in
small to moderate negative correlations with the Flexibility Index
and Verbal Fluency. Small negative correlations were found between
Statue and teacher ratings on the Flexibility Index and Emerging
Metacognition Index. Post hoc analysis of Clinical scale ratings
revealed additional correlations. Small to moderate negative
correlations were found between teacher ratings on the Shift and
Plan/Organize Clinical scales and all four NEPSY subtests. Small to
moderate negative correlations were found between parent ratings on
the Shift and Emotional Control Clinical scales and the Visual
Attention and Verbal Fluency subtests. Overall, this study suggests
the amount of variance shared between test performance and behavior
ratings is small, giving support to a multidimensional evaluation
approach.
Rossen, E.
A., Shearer, D. K., Penfield, R. D., &Kranzler, J. H. (2005).
Validity of the Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligence
(CTONI). Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment,
23(2), 161- 172.(click to
view)
Abstract. This study examined the construct
validity of the Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligence
(CTONI) in two separate investigations. The first study examined
criterionrelated evidence of validity across racial/ethnic groups
on the CTONI and the Woodcock- Johnson Psychoeducational Tests of
Achievement (3rd edition). The second study examined the
comparability of the standard and computerized forms of the CTONI.
Results of the first study revealed that the CTONI overall score
did not correlate significantly with reading achievement and
correlated moderately with math achievement. The CTONI also showed
significant racial/ethnic group differences, despite the absence of
these differences on achievement. Results of the second study
revealed that average scores on the standard form of the CTONI were
significantly higher statistically than those on the computerized
form. Correlations between raw scores on the computerized form of
the CTONI and age were statistically significant, although
generally low. In sum, results of this research raise concerns and
questions about the validity of the CTONI. The CTONI should be used
with caution, if at all, until further research is
conducted.
Sherman,
M. R. (2002). Structural modeling of the WAIS-R and tests of
executive function under comprehensive CHC
theory.Dissertation Abstracts International Section A:
Humanities & Social Sciences, 63(1-A), 87.
Abstract: Tests of executive function are frequently
administered in neuropsychological assessment batteries to evaluate
abilities such as organization, planning, concept formation, set
shifting, and response inhibition. Although executive dysfunction
is thought to cause academic and vocational difficulties, school
psychologists and educational diagnosticians typically do not
evaluate executive functioning as part of a standard assessment
battery. Recently, several psychometric batteries purporting to
measure executive processes (e.g. the Cognitive Assessment System,
the NEPSY, and the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive
Ability-Third Edition) have been published. Many of the measures on
these batteries are newer versions of traditional
neuropsychological assessment instruments. Controversy over what
these newer tests of executive functioning actually measure has
arisen in the literature. The Cattell-Horn- Carroll (CHC) cross-
battery approach to assessment, developed by McGrew and Flanagan,
is a structured multiple intelligence theory that provides a
framework to conduct psychoeducational assessments. Their approach
calls for systematic administration and interpretation of various
subtests from different batteries to more thoroughly and accurately
assess cognitive abilities. The purpose of this study is to examine
the factor structure of tests of executive function according to
Comprehensive CHC theory. A confirmatory factor analysis was
conducted using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, the Stroop Color
and Word Test, the Trail Making Test, the Rey- Osterrieth Complex
Figure Test, and selected subtests from the Wechsler Adult
Intelligence Scale- Revised. Three models representing CHC
classifications and the WAIS-R three factor structure were proposed
for comparison. Results of the study found that the best fitting
model was conceptualized using CHC classifications of both the
executive function tests and the WAIS-R subtests. The results
suggest that executive functions can be examined by providing
content- matched control tasks; CHC theory provides a framework to
develop and assess content- matched controls for executive function
tasks. Implications for school psychologists and educational
diagnosticians are proposed.
Titus, J. B. (2002). The prediction of
cognitive ability from sensory/motor performance: Examining the
role of sensory/motor performance in the Dean-Woodcock Cognitive
Neuropsychology Model. Dissertation Abstracts
International: Section B: The Sciences & Engineering,
63((6-B)), 3058.
Abstract: The present study investigated the
relationship between sensory/motor skills and cognitive abilities
in psychiatric and neurologically impaired patients to determine
how sensory/motor skills are associated with cognitive abilities.
Previous research has demonstrated a significant relationship
between performance on sensory/motor tasks and cognition but has
failed to examine how well sensory/motor skills can predict
specific cognitive performance. Because brain functioning is
hierarchical with simpler processes being foundational to the
development of more complex functions, it is likely sensory/motor
skills can aid in the prediction of specific cognitive abilities.
If this is true, then investigation of the relationship between
sensory/motor performance and cognition should provide insight into
the role of sensory/motor skills in the Dean- Woodcock Cognitive
Neuropsychology Model. This study examined the relationship between
scores on the Woodcock- Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability-Revised
(WJ-R COG) and scores on the Dean-Woodcock Sensory and Motor
Battery (D- WSMB). Participants included 458 patients referred for
neuropsychological consultation with neurological and/or
psychiatric diagnoses. Results indicated a significant correlation
among cognitive scores and sensory/motor scores. Moreover, factor
analysis revealed 7 overall factors that account for approximately
65% of the variance. These factors were identified as being
thinking ability and processing speed, subcortical functioning,
verbal working memory, peripheral processes, tactile- kinesthetic
thinking (Gtk), visual processing, and simple sensory/motor.
Integration of sensory/motor factors into the Dean- Woodcock
Cognitive Neuropsychology Model revealed the significant role
sensory/motor performance plays in predicting higher- order
cognitive abilities. From the analysis it appeared that certain
sensory/motor functions were significant contributors to the
prediction of specific cognitive abilities. That is, subcortical
functioning aided in the prediction of all measured areas of
cognition; visual processing contributed to visual- spatial
thinking, novel reasoning, and crystallized knowledge;
tactile-kinesthetic thinking helped predict visual-spatial
thinking, auditory processing, and crystallized knowledge; and
simple sensory/motor functioning aided in the explanation of long-
term storage- retrieval. These results offer a beginning point for
further investigation into the relationship between specific
sensory/motor skills and cognitive abilities. Research in this area
can provide further insight into the functional organization of the
brain and offer application to cognitive outcome in rehabilitation
as well as preschool screening.
Valbuena,
J. P. (2004). Concurrent validity of the ability/achievement
discrepancy identified by the Bilingual Verbal Ability Score -
Woodcock Johnson-revised tests of
Achievement method. Dissertation Abstracts International:
Section B: The Sciences & Engineering, 64(11- B),
5836.
Abstract: In 1975, Public Law 94-142, also known as
The Education of All Handicapped Children Act, established the
rights of the bilingual student to a nondiscriminatory assessment
and called for a systematic plan to address any deficits identified
by the assessment. Since that time and through the reauthorization
of that law, now known as the Individuals with Disabilities
Education Act, those charged with the evaluation of cognitive and
achievement skills of children and adolescents have struggled to
comply with the mandate. One of the challenges they face is the
dearth of well designed, reliable and valid instruments to conduct
evaluations of the growing number of bilingual, limited English
proficient students. The Bilingual Verbal Ability Tests are
purported to be unique measures of general cognitive ability in
that they incorporate skills in both English and a second language.
The current study attempted to assess the validity of the
ability/achievement discrepancy identified by the Bilingual Verbal
Ability Score/Woodcock- Johnson - Revised Tests of Achievement
method. This method compares predicted ability, estimated with the
Bilingual Verbal Ability Score, with actual ability as measured by
the Woodcock-Johnson - Revised Tests of Achievement. The task
is complicated by the fact that no other instrument has been
designed to include the impact of bilingual skills on cognitive
abilities. The ability/achievement discrepancy identified by the
more traditional Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children -
III/Wechsler Individual Achievement Test method was used as the
criterion measure. The most important outcome of the current study
was observed among subjects whose second language skills did not
significantly impact their BVA score. Within that group, for
subtests measuring reading skills and comprehension, the study
revealed moderate to high correlations between the levels of
discrepancy identified by the two methods. The impact of these
results is limited by the small, skewed sample but suggests that
the usefulness of the information provided by the Bilingual Verbal
Ability Tests warrants its inclusion in the standard battery of
instruments administered in the course of an assessment of learning
abilities.
|
|