In a recent behavioral study,Nakata and Mitani
(2005) demonstrated, that even infants between 6 and 8 months of
age are able to distinguish between regular and irregular tone
sequences. When testing the average looking times for colored
flashes, combined either with regular or irregular sound sequences,
infants paid more attention to the flashes when the onset-onset
distance was constant (173 ms), but not when this distance was
variable.
In the framework of scalar timing theory, usually
considered to be the most completely developed model of timing
(Allan, 1998; Droit-Volet & Wearden, 2003), recent studies
have investigated how temporal discrimination in children changes
with age by using the temporal bisection method initially used
in animals (Church & Deluty, 1977) and later modified for human
adults (Allan & Gibbon, 1991; Wearden, 1991). In the bisection
procedure commonly used with human adults, participants receive
initial presentations of two standard durations, a short and
a long standard duration. Then they are presented with
comparison durations including these two standards as well as
intermediate stimulus durations. Their task is to classify each
presented duration as being more similar to the short or to the
long standard duration.
Furthermore, by using different duration ranges,
Droit-Volet and Wearden (2001, 2002) succeeded in showing that
young children’s bisection performance exhibits the scalar
property—that is, the requirement that the standard
deviation of time judgements increases in proportion to the mean of
the duration judged. These different results demonstrate that
young children possess a fundamental ability to represent time,
similar to that found in animals and human adults. Therefore,
Droit-Volet and her colleagues concluded that a
pacemaker–accumulator clock mechanism underlying time
representations, like that proposed by scalar timing theory, is
functional at an early age (Droit-Volet, 2002; Droit-Volet,
Clément, & Wearden, 2001; Droit-Volet & Wearden, 2001,
2002). However, although young children are able to represent time,
some age-related changes have been observed in their temporal
discrimination behaviour on bisection tasks. For example, the
steepness of the slope of the psychophysical functions increased
with age, the functions being flatter in the 3- and the 5-year-olds
than in the 8-year-olds (for a review, see Droit-Volet, 2003). A
measure of this steepness of the psychophysical function and,
consequently, an index of temporal sensitivity is the Weber
ratio.
In young children, the greater sensitivity to
duration for the auditory than for the visual stimuli suggests
a sort of a dominance of audition over vision in the processing of
time.
In conclusion, our results showed that the
auditory stimuli were judged longer than the visual stimuli
whatever the age group tested and that this was probably due to the
faster clock rate for the auditory stimuli. Thus, these results
provided support for the idea that the modality differences in the
pacemaker speed were present at an early age. The
age-related changes in the modality effect on time judgements
would be explained by the temporal sensitivity that was lower for
the visual than for the auditory stimuli in young children, related
to the development of attentional control that itself affects the
variance of the switch latency of the internal clock.