Few agree on whether temporal processing is
centralized or distributed and which structures are involved.
Indeed, if all neural circuits can intrinsically process
temporal information, then virtually any circuit could be involved,
and the location of temporal processing would depend on the nature
and modality of the task at hand.
The holy grail of timing research is to
understand the ‘time-dependent process’: a
mechanism equivalent to a piezoelectric crystal in a man-made clock
or the movement of a shadow on a sundial. This has proven an
elusive goal, to the extent that ideas about how this mechanism
might work remain near the level of conjecture. Researchers have
had great difficulty in pinning timing- related activity in the
brain to any specific type of function. This is largely because
mosttime measurement tasks draw upon more than one process,
making it difficult to tease the various components
apart.
Circadian, interval and millisecond timing
involve different neural mechanisms. In mammals, the circadian
clock that drives metabolic and behavioural rhythms is located in
the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) of the hypothalamus. This master
clock coordinates tissue-specific rhythms according to light input
and other cues — such as social information — that it
receives from the outside world. The circadian timer relies on a
molecular network of transcriptional feedback loops. On the other
hand, interval timing depends on the intact striatum, but not on
the intact SCN or cerebellum. In the interval- timing range, the
striatum and the cerebellum might both be activated, possibly
contributing to different aspects of performance as a function of
the sequential stages of motor memory consolidation.
Brain responses in cognitive neuroscience reveal
that the integration of successive auditory items already takes
place at a very early processing stage, that is, in the first 200
ms after stimulus- onset. The process of integration is
reflected by an early negative component of the event-related
potential (ERP), the so-called mismatch negativity (MMN;
e.g., Sussman,Winkler, Ritter, Alho, & Näätänen,
1999). In the auditory domain, the MMN is evoked whenever a change
within a flow of repetitive stimuli can be identified. If, for
example, a stream of constant auditory events (e.g., tones or short
words) is given, the brain detects this regularity and forms a so-
called standard. If this standard trace is violated
by a deviant sound, an MMN is elicited (for a review,
see, e.g., Näätänen, Tervaniemi, Sussman,
Paavilainen, & Winkler, 2001). If stimuli are presented very
rapidly, they are even more strongly integrated and form an
auditory object that is perceived as a whole (Sussman, Ritter,
& Vaughan, 1998).